Free Essay

Thesis

In:

Submitted By jemimah
Words 7396
Pages 30
International Journal of Psychophysiology 49 (2003) 187–199

Are poor readers semantically challenged? An event-related brain potential assessment
´
´
´
Juan Silva-Pereyraa,b, *, Maritza Rivera-Gaxiolab, Thalıa Fernandeza, Lourdes Dıaz-Comasc,
´
´
´
Thalıa Harmonya, Antonio Fernandez-Bouzasa,d, Mario Rodrıguezd, Jorge Bernald,
Erszebet Marosid b a
´
´
´
Instituto de Neurobiologıa, Universidad Nacional Autonoma de Mexico, UNAM-UAQ, Queretaro, Mexico
Center for Mind, Brain and Learning, University of Washington, 358 Fisheries Center Box 357988, Seattle, WA 98195, USA c Centro de Neurociencias, La Habana, Cuba d ´
´
Neurociencias ENEP Iztacala, Universidad Nacional Autonoma de Mexico, Mexico City, Mexico

Received 8 October 2002; received in revised form 6 May 2003; accepted 13 May 2003

Abstract
This study explores visual event-related potentials components in a group of poor readers (PRs) and control children who carried out figure and word categorization tasks. In both tasks, every child had to categorize between animal and non-animal stimuli in an odd-ball GO–GO paradigm. During the word categorization task, PRs presented longer reaction times, a poorer performance, longer and larger P2 amplitudes, and smaller amplitudes and longer
P300 latencies than controls. There were no differences in the N400 component between groups. These results suggest that semantic processing underachievement in PRs may not be a semantic deficit per se, but the late reflection of an early word codification problem, deficient use of attentional resources and lack of target identification during reading.
2003 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Poor readers, semantic processing, N400, P300, P2; Categorization tasks; Event-related potentials, reading disabled children 1. Introduction
Children who score between 1 and 2 years below their expected reading level have been considered to be poor readers (PRs) (Rayner and
Pollatsek, 1989). Children scoring 2 or more years below their expected reading level are referred to as dyslexics. In both cases, children have IQ scores
*Corresponding author. Tel.: q1-206-2216471; fax: q1206-2216472.
E-mail address: jfsp@u.washington.edu (J. Silva-Pereyra).

that fall within the normal range. One of the main factors involved in the development of reading skills is to create a transparent interface between orthographic input and phonological structure.
Phonological skills that are required to segment words into their constituent phonemes not developing normally, very likely give rise to poor reading. However, it is unlikely that all reading problems are solely due to this factor. Among the prime candidates that have also been proposed are deficits in syntactic processing (Byrne, 1981),

0167-8760/03/$ - see front matter
2003 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/S0167-8760(03)00116-8 188

J. Silva-Pereyra et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 49 (2003) 187–199

limitations in working memory capacity (Swanson,
1992; Swanson and Sachse, 2001) and impairments in the time course of the processing of the rapid, transient characteristics of auditory and visual input (Tallal et al., 1993).
Our approach to the present study was to obtain behavioral data while simultaneously recording event-related potentials (ERPs) from electrodes placed over different regions of the scalp. ERPs reflect the stimulus-locked cognitive information processing activities in terms of distributed assemblies of neurons firing synchronized across time.
Thus, different patterns of brain electrical activity recorded during a task may reflect different cognitive processes and the differences in amplitude and latency of the ERP components across different populations may represent cognitive abilities and y or disabilities.
One ERP component that has traditionally been studied in PRs is the so-called P300 component.
The P300 wave is an endogenous positive deflection with an average peak latency at 300 ms or more after stimulus onset and is typically elicited by a rarely and randomly presented target stimulus in a detection task. One prominent interpretation of the P300 is that it represents stimulus evaluation and memory updating (Donchin and Coles, 1988;
Polich and Kok, 1995). ERP studies have shown smaller amplitudes and longer latencies of the
P300 component in PRs (Diniz et al., 1997;
Holcomb et al., 1985; Silva-Pereyra et al., 2001) and dyslexics (Taylor and Keenan, 1990). Some other studies have found that PRs have lower scores than controls during working memory tasks
(De Jong, 1998; Isaki and Plante, 1997). Holcomb et al. (1985) found that reading disabled and control subjects could be better differentiated from one another by the amplitude of the P300 when non-target word stimuli were contrasted with nonlinguistic symbols: symbols elicited larger P300 than words in reading disabled children, while the two kinds of stimuli elicited equivalent amplitudes in controls. There is also evidence that PRs have longer P300 latencies during a color discrimination task (Silva-Pereyra et al., 2001). Some authors have suggested that the long P300 latency of dyslexic and reading disabled children is due to the fact, that in these children, stimulus evaluation

and memory updating processes take more time
(Holcomb et al., 1985). Early auditory and visual sensory processing deficits also seem to be an important feature in the profile of reading disabled children. Such deficits apparently lead to compensatory increases in the effort required to integrate words into a context and to a greater reliance on context for word recognition than control subjects
(Neville et al., 1993). Several studies have found larger P2 amplitudes to a surprising sound in reading disabled children (Bernal et al., 2000;
Iragui et al., 1993). Holcomb et al. (1986) also found larger P2 amplitudes to unexpected stimuli in reading disabled and in children suffering attentional deficit with hyperactivity when compared to controls. The P2 is a component that has been associated with reallocation of attentional resources during task performance (Luck and Hillyard, 1994).
The design used in the present experiment implies that subjects make semantic category decisions with different probabilities of stimulus in a word (or figure)-list presentation. The decision process could produce a large P3 in addition to a previous N400 (Halgren, 1990; Hill et al., 2002;
Nielsen-Bohlman and Knight, 1994; Polich, 1985;
Stelmack et al., 1988). The P3 usually overlaps the N400, and is also sensitive to priming, and manipulations of expectancy (Deacon et al., 2000).
However, some studies have shown that this overlapping is only partial (Chwilla et al., 1995; Kutas and van Petten, 1994) or not at all affected by the
P3 component (Nobre and McCarthy, 1994). The
N400 has been associated with semantic processing and it is a negative-going wave between 300 and 600 ms, peaking approximately at 400 ms after stimulus onset, with a posterior, slightly right amplitude predominant asymmetry (Kutas and
Hillyard, 1980). The N400 effect has been observed in sentence (Kutas and Hillyard, 1980) and priming paradigms (Bentin et al., 1985).
Sentence processing developmental studies (Holcomb et al., 1992; Juottonen et al., 1996) have shown that with age, the N400 effect decreases in amplitude and increases in latency. These results suggest that as children acquire better language skills, they rely less on semantic context for language comprehension.

J. Silva-Pereyra et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 49 (2003) 187–199

In contrast to the previous, the N400 elicited during word-list tasks without priming has a more anterior distribution and is the largest at central and frontal locations (especially the midline)
(Nobre and McCarthy, 1994). Despite the distributional difference, the anterior N400 is thought to be closely related to the classic N400 and probably reflects similar cognitive processes. In the present study, during word categorization task, a similar anterior N400 is anticipated.
On the other hand, picture-processing paradigms also show an N400 effect that may mirror the pattern found in word-prime studies (Barret and
Rugg, 1990; Holcomb and McPherson, 1994). The
N400-like effect elicited by pictures is the result of both a frontally distributed N300 and a more widely distributed N450 (Barret and Rugg, 1990).
There is no evidence of amplitude change with age on picture processing development, although latency decreases with increasing age (Friedman et al., 1990). Although it has been suggested that the N450 could be related with the processing of semantic relationships between non-verbal stimuli, other studies comparing picture with word semantic processing using ERPs have suggested that the semantic analysis of words and pictures may involve different, at least partially non-overlapping, neural systems (Holcomb and McPherson,
1994).
While there is abundant and convergent evidence showing that PRs tend to be deficient in phonologically based skills (Beitchman and
Young, 1997) or working memory capacity (Swanson, 1992; Swanson and Sachse, 2001), there is also evidence of a deficiency in semantically based skills (Champion, 1997; Ben-Dror et al., 1995;
Gillon and Dodd, 1994; Stelmack et al., 1988;
Vellutino et al., 1988). Some studies have reported differences favoring normal readers in semantic memory as well as semantic learning tasks using visual symbols and paired associates (Vellutino and Scanlon, 1985; Vellutino et al., 1995; Waterman and Lewandowski, 1993). Stelmack et al.
(1988) found larger frontal N400 amplitudes for normal readers with respect to reading disabled children during word recognition. They believe that this effect is consistent with a more extensive

189

semantic evaluation or memory search attributed to that component.
In summary, deficiencies in semantic tasks in
PRs may be produced by a less effective processing of stimuli, taking more time for their evaluation, which may be reflected by a smaller and later
P300. Although reading disabled children have difficulties with the translation of visual information into a speech code, they also appear to have deficits in the classification and memory processing of the non-verbal visual stimuli (Silva-Pereyra et al., 2001). This deficiency in early processing may also affect semantic processing in PRs (Stelmack et al., 1988). In an attempt to evaluate the access to semantic information independently of stimulus kind, in the present study, categorization tasks of animal y non-animal presented as figures and words were used.
2. Materials and methods
2.1. Subjects
Thirty-four children participated in this study.
They were selected from a group of third and fourth grade volunteers from two elementary schools. Children were divided into two groups according to an evaluation carried out by three of the authors, based on interviews with parents and child, the teacher’s opinion and academic achievement (18 controls and 16 PRs). In order to evaluate subject allocation into each group, a battery for the computerized analysis of reading in Spanish was
´
applied to each child. This is the ‘Baterıa para los
Trastornos de la Lectura’ (BTL, Reading Disabilities Battery, Reigosa et al., 1994).
The mean age for the control group was 10.1 years (1.6 S.D.) and 10.2 years (1.9 S.D.) for
PRs. The male:female ratio was 1:1 for normal readers and 2.5:1 for PRs. None of the children had a history of school problems or a neurological disorder. They were all right-handed and had a normal neurological examination, as well as normal IQ scores (WISC-R in Spanish; mean IQEs
106.33 S.D.s14.68, mean IQVs105.11 S.D.s
8.81 for controls; mean IQEs100.81 S.D.s12.36, mean IQVs98.81 S.D.s14 for PRs). Comparison

190

J. Silva-Pereyra et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 49 (2003) 187–199

between groups using the IQ scores showed no significant differences.

Each figure was presented for 2000 ms and the interval (ISI) between figures was 3000 ms.

2.2. Stimuli

2.3.2. W task
This task was analogous to the F task in requirements, except that full words instead of figures were displayed. Correct and incorrect responses were automatically marked in the electroencephalogram (EEG) recording.

The 260 figures presented were obtained from
Snodgrass and Vanderwart’s study (1980) and were digitized and displayed on the center of a computer video. The figures were white line drawings on a black background, with a 3.12=3.12 degrees visual angle. In order to obtain the list of words corresponding to the figures, a pilot study was carried out with another sample of 15 children.
After presenting the figures, each child had to say the first word that came to mind. All words produced by each of the 15 children were stored and analyzed for frequency of occurrence and the corresponding percentage. The highest frequency word for each figure was selected. In this way, a list of 201 words produced by figure nomination and the corresponding figures was created.
Two categorization tasks were designed: a figure categorization (F task) and a word categorization task (W task). In the W task, the words employed were obtained from the nomination of the 201 figures to be used in the F task. The words of this list were displayed in 1-cm uppercase letters in the center of a 14-inch computer monitor (white letters on a black screen). At the viewing distance employed, each letter subtended a visual angle of approximately 0.573=0.573 degrees.
2.3. Procedure
2.3.1. F task
All 201 figure trials were randomly presented:
44 figures were animals and 157 were non-animals
(household items, utensils, tools, personal objects, fruits and vegetables and vehicles). Participants were instructed to respond by pressing one button of the computer keyboard (letter B) with their index finger if the stimulus presented was an animal and a different button (letter M) with the middle finger if it was not; in a GO–GO task. The use of buttons was counterbalanced across subjects. Children were instructed to respond as rapidly and accurately as possible to each stimulus.

2.3.3. EEG recording
The EEG was recorded with Ag y AgCl electrodes referenced to linked earlobes from Fp1,
Fp2, F3, F4, C3, C4, P3, P4, O1, O2, F7, F8, T3,
T4, T5, T6, Fz, Cz, Pz and Oz of the 10 y 20 international system. The amplifier bandwidth was set between 0.05 and 30 Hz. All electrode impedances were at or below 5 kV and amplified with a gain of 20 000. EEG was sampled every 5 ms and stored on a hard disk for further analysis. EEG segments of 1280 ms with a pre-stimulus time of
100 ms were selected and averaged off-line to obtain the ERPs. The electro-oculogram (EOG) was recorded from a supra-orbital electrode and from an electrode placed at the external canthus of the right eye. Special care was taken to reject segments with eye movements or other artifacts.
In addition, trials on which EEG or EOG activity exceeded "75 mV were rejected. Baseline correction was performed in relation to a pre-stimulus time of 100 ms.
2.4. Data analysis
2.4.1. Behavioral data
The median of reaction times (RTs) for correct responses from each subject was calculated and the data used to perform a three-way ANOVA.
The variables included were: group (PRs and controls), task (F and W) and category (animal and non-animal). A second three-way ANOVA was performed using percentage of error. Tukey honest significant difference post hoc tests were done. 2.4.2. ERP data
ERPs from correct responses by task (F, W) and by category (animal, non-animal) were obtained.

J. Silva-Pereyra et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 49 (2003) 187–199

191

Fig. 1. The top figure shows RT mean values from the group=task and group=category interactions. PRs showed longer RT during the W task, even this difference was larger to non-animals category. The bottom figure shows proportion of error interaction effect
(task=category=group). Differences were observed between PRs and controls on the animal category in the word categorization task. Significant differences are marked with an asterisk (P-0.05).

Approximately an equal number of EEG segments
(f25 segments) were taken to average by experimental condition across subjects.
According to their appearance in the grand average waveforms, the P2 peak was defined as the most positive voltage within the interval of
200–300 ms, the N400 peak was defined as the most negative voltage within the interval of 400–
600 ms and the P300 as the most positive value within the interval of 600–900 ms.
2.4.3. Amplitude
Separate five-way ANOVAs were carried out for each component using the following variables: group=task=category=hemisphere (left and right)=anterior–posterior (Frontal F3–F4, frontotemporal F7–F8, Central C3–C4, Parietal P3–P4, anterior temporal T3–T4, posterior temporal T5–
T6 and Occipital O1–O2). The Huynh–Feldt epsilon was applied to the degrees of freedom of those analyses with more than one degree of freedom in the numerator. Tukey honest significant difference

post hoc tests were done. Because the latency of the P2 varies across the head, the data were analyzed using ten electrode positions (Fp1–Fp2,
F3–F4, C3–C4, T3–T4 and F7–F8).
2.4.4. Latency
Separate three-way ANOVAs for each component were done using latency measurements at Cz.
The variables were group (PRs and controls)=task
(figures and words)=category (animals vs. nonanimals). Tukey honest significant difference post hoc tests were done.
3. Results
3.1. Behavioral results
3.1.1. Reaction times
There were significant group=task (F(1, 32)s
8.3 Ps0.007) and group=category (F(1, 32)s
10.05 Ps0.003) interactions. The first interaction revealed significantly longer RTs for PRs in the

192

J. Silva-Pereyra et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 49 (2003) 187–199

W task (Ps0.0003) (see left-top of Fig. 1). The second one shows longer RTs for PRs than controls to animals (Ps0.001) and to non-animals (Ps
0.0002) (right-top of Fig. 1).
3.1.2. Percentage of error
Regarding percentage of errors, there was a significant group=task interaction (F(1, 32)s
8.94 Ps0.005), indicating a higher percentage of error in PRs during the W task (Ps0.0003). There was also a significant group=task=category interaction (F(1, 32)s7.35 Ps0.001). PRs may have therefore been less efficient than controls in categorizing when the stimulus was an animal during the W task (Ps0.0002) (Fig. 1).
3.1.3. ERP results
For both groups and tasks, three components were visible. The first major positive peak, most pronounced over anterior sites, began approximately at 175 ms during post-stimulus onset and reached its maximum amplitude at 240 ms. The next component was a negative wave of approximately 400 ms. This negativity was larger to the non-animal than to the animal category. This negativity was more visible in the F task over anterior sites, and in both groups; during the W task, it showed two peaks with 100 ms of difference (Fig.
2). The third major component was a positive waveform which was more prominent at posterior regions of approximately 600 and 90 ms. This peak was larger to infrequent stimuli (animal) than to frequent stimuli (non-animals).
3.2. P2
3.2.1. Amplitude
The P2 was distributed over the scalp with a most pronounced amplitude over frontal areas
(main effect of electrode site F(2.6, 82.4)s47.7
P-0.0001), and the left hemisphere (hemisphere main effect (1, 32)s10.7 Ps0.003). It was larger in amplitude for the W than for the F task (main effect of task (F(1, 32)s80.18 P-0.00001). The distribution of the P2 during the F with respect to the W task was different (task=electrode site interaction F(2.7, 85.73)s8.53 P-0.0001). During the F task, the P2 could be seen on fronto-

polar (Fp1–Fp2), frontal (F3, F4, F7, F8) and central (C3, C4, T3, T4) sites. On the other hand, during the W task, the distribution was frontal
(Fp1, Fp2, F3, F4), central (C3, C4, F7, F8) and temporal (T3, T4).
There was a significant group=task interaction
(F(1, 32)s8.22 Ps0.007), that is, PRs showed larger P2 than controls during the W task (Ps
0.03). PRs also showed larger P2 amplitudes than controls (Ps0.003) on the left hemisphere for animals (group=category=hemisphere interaction
F(1, 32)s5.7 Ps0.023).
Finally, there was a significant group=hemisphere=electrode site interaction (F(2.7, 86.3)s
2.86 Ps0.047; Fig. 3), indicating that PRs had larger P2 amplitudes on Fp1 (P-0.0001), F3 (Ps
0.0002) and F7 (P-0.0001), whilst the control group showed larger amplitudes than PRs on T4
(Ps0.02).
3.2.2. Latency
Significantly longer P2 latencies during the W than during the F task (main effect of task F(1,
32)s5.62 Ps0.024) and for animals than nonanimal (main effect of category F(1, 32)s11.96
Ps0.0016) were observed in both groups.
PRs showed longer P2 latencies than Controls
(Ps0.0013) during the W task (group=task interaction F(1, 32)s15.1 Ps0.0005; Fig. 3). PRs also showed longer latencies than controls (Ps
0.0007) for the non-animal category (group=category interaction F(1, 32)s6.9 Ps0.01). Finally, there was a significant group=task=category interaction (F(1, 32)s11.6 Ps0.002): Controls had longer latencies than PRs (Ps0.001) for animals during the F task. On the other hand, PRs showed longer P240 latencies than controls for animals (Ps0.0002) and non-animals (Ps
0.0009) during the W task.
3.3. N400
3.3.1. Amplitude
There were no significant main effects of group or interactions by group. However, there was a significant main effect of task (F(1, 32)s20.8
P-0.001) which means larger N400 amplitudes for the F than the W task. This difference was

J. Silva-Pereyra et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 49 (2003) 187–199

193

Fig. 2. This figure shows: (a) grand average ERPs to figures (animal and non-animal) for PRs and controls; (b) ERPs to words
(animal and non-animal) for both groups. Larger P2 amplitudes and smaller P300 amplitude for PRs during the word categorization task were observed. Two cursors on both N400 peaks are shown at Cz from 350 to 560 ms

194

J. Silva-Pereyra et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 49 (2003) 187–199

Fig. 3. This figure shows: (a) group=category=hemisphere interaction for the P2 amplitudes. The PRs shows larger P2 for the left hemisphere but for the animal category there were significant differences; (b) group=task=category interaction for the P2 latency measurements. PRs display longer P2 latencies during W task. Significant differences are marked with an asterisk (P-0.05).

seen over frontal (F3–F4 P-0.0001), frontotemporal (F7–F8 Ps0.003) and parietal (P3–P4
P-0.0001) regions (task=electrode site interaction F(2.8, 90.3)s21.33 P-0.001).
There was a significant task=category= electrode site interaction (F(2.13, 68.25)s5.66
Ps0.005), which means that there was an N400 effect in both groups, but only during the F task on posterior temporal (T5–T6, Ps0.0009) and occipital regions (O1–O2, Ps0.00002).
3.3.2. Latency
There were no significant effects.
3.4. P300
3.4.1. Amplitude
There was a significant group=task=category
=hemisphere interaction (F(1, 32)s5.99 Ps

0.02). The P300 effect (larger amplitudes for animals than non-animals) was observed for controls on the left hemisphere during the F (Ps
0.002) and the W tasks (Ps0.0006). Controls also showed this P300 effect on the right hemisphere during both tasks (F, Ps0.04 and W, Ps0.02). In contrast, PRs showed a P300 effect on the left hemisphere only during the F task (Ps0.0002;
Fig. 4). The topographical distribution of this effect was different regarding task and group
(group=task=category=electrode site F(2.26,
72.45)s2.98 Ps0.05). In the control group, the
P300 effect was significant on parietal (P0.0001), posterior temporal (Ps0.0001) and occipital (P-0.0001) areas during the F task.
Controls also showed a significant P300 effect on central electrodes (Ps0.0005), parietal (P0.0001), posterior temporal (Ps0.0001) and occipital (P-0.0001) during the W task. In contrast, PRs showed a significant P300 effect during
F task on central (Ps0.014), parietal (P0.0001), posterior temporal (P-0.0001) and occipital (P-0.0001), but there was P300 effect during W task.
3.4.2. Latency
Latency differences between animals and nonanimals were bigger in both groups only during the F (task=category F(1, 32)s7.26 Ps0.01).
PRs showed significantly longer P300 latencies
(Ps0.03) during the W task (group=task interaction F(1, 32)s4.83 Ps0.035; Fig. 4).
4. Discussion
In the present study, animal y non-animal figures’ and words’ categorization tasks elicited three ERP components in PRs and control subjects: P2, N400 and P300. An anterior P2 was seen on fronto-polar and frontal leads and more pronounced over the left hemisphere mainly during the W task. Following the P2 component, a frontally negative waveform of approximately 445 ms and a posterior negativity of approximately 520 ms were clear during the F task. A dual-negative-going, anteriorly maximal, but widely distributed negative peak between 350 and 560 ms in the W task was evident. At posterior sites a positive-going com-

J. Silva-Pereyra et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 49 (2003) 187–199

195

Fig. 4. This figure shows: (a) group=task=category=hemisphere interaction for the P300 amplitudes; (b) group=task interaction for the P300 latency measurements. PRs display longer P300 latencies and smaller amplitudes during W task. Significant differences are marked with an asterisk (P-0.05).

ponent peaking approximately at 750 ms (P300) was present and largest for the animal condition which has the lowest presentation probability. This peak was practically absent in PRs during the word categorization task.
In order to compare both groups exclusively at the semantic level, it is of great importance to assume that the tasks are equivalent in cognitive terms. It could be argued, for example, that in order to categorize figures, subjects rely on shape similarity and other physical attributes, which could facilitate a successful performance (Snodgrass and McCullough, 1986). In such a case, one should obtain shorter RTs and y or earlier latencies of semantic related late ERP components during the F task in both groups. If anything, the results of the present study showed only earlier P2 latencies in the F task than in the W task in both groups. We argue that this difference could reflect

a lower complexity in stimulus’ evaluation (Taylor and Khan, 2000) to access the semantic system, but not semantic differences per se. In this way, we believe the comparison between tasks is valid.
PRs presented longer RTs and a higher percentage of errors with respect to the control group during the word categorization task. Phonological processes such as accessing a word by first decoding the written symbols into sound representations and phonological coding to maintain information in working memory have been implicated in reading failure (Beitchman and Young, 1997; Wagner and Torgesen, 1987). Deficiencies at the phonological level interfere at least with the activation of word meaning or slow down the semantic information access (Zecker and Zinner, 1987). It has also been suggested that PRs could primarily activate the meanings of words using the phonological route, which could make the whole process

196

J. Silva-Pereyra et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 49 (2003) 187–199

even slower. Good readers, on the other hand, primarily activate the meanings of words using the direct route (i.e. without the translation processing from grapheme to phoneme) (Jared et al., 1999).
This evidence could therefore explain the lower performance of our PRs group during the W task.
Our results showed that PRs have a larger and longer anterior P2 than controls during the word categorization task. This effect was also seen more pronounced over the left hemisphere and on frontopolar and frontal leads. The anterior P2 peak has been associated with attentional demands (Johnson, 1989) and stimulus evaluation (Luck and
Hillyard, 1994). Its amplitude decreases with age
(Taylor and Khan, 2000) but increases with the difficulty level of the task (Stelmack et al., 1988;
Taylor and Khan, 2000). Right)Left posterior P2 hemisphere asymmetries have been related to better reading skills (Segalowitz et al., 1992) and
L)R anterior P2 asymmetries have been found in children at risk for reading difficulties (Khan et al., 1999). Although there is a controversy about distributional ERP component differences between poor and normal readers, the lateralization of semantic processing with increased age has also been previously reported (Holcomb et al., 1992).
Our results clearly showed L)R P2 asymmetries in the PRs group and, in agreement with Khan et al. (1999) we claim that this group employs less mature strategies in the word processing; even the allocation of more attentional resources could be the expression of an inappropriate strategy.
The maturation of the prefrontal cortex is extremely important during cognitive development
(Stauder et al., 1993) and therefore, reading. Segalowitz et al. (1992) argue that PRs may have difficulty with attentional requirements for visual processing and metacognitive strategies associated with reading, which place a demand on the frontal system. According to Taylor and Smith (1995), the latency of the P2 component decreases with age. This developmental trend could mean that children become faster or that they have more efficient strategies to evaluate stimuli with increasing age. Khan et al. (1999) observed longer anterior P2 latencies on a group of children at risk for reading difficulties. Our PRs also showed longer P2 latencies which may mirror the use of

an immature stimulus evaluation strategy and a deficit to reallocate attentional resources.
In contrast to the previous ERP component results, there were no clear significant differences between groups regarding the N400 component.
Both groups showed an anterior negativity of approximately 445 ms and a posterior negativity of approximately 520 ms during the figure categorization task. These two negativities were previously reported during picture priming (Barret and Rugg, 1990) and picture list studies (Coch et al., 2002). Both groups also showed a more pronounced anterior N400 during figure categorizations than word categorizations. These amplitude topographical distribution differences could show that different cognitive components may be involved in the figure and word semantic processing (Holcomb and McPherson, 1994). The point we wish to emphasize here is that PRs showed no differences in their performance and N400 component with respect to the controls during the figure categorization task where they can easily access the semantic information provided by these stimuli. Both groups showed a negative dual-peak during the word categorization task. Prior studies have shown that stimuli presented in list form tend to elicit such anteriorly distributed negativities
(Nobre and McCarthy, 1994). Approximately, the first peak was observed at 350 ms and the second at 560 ms, they were mainly distributed on C3,
Cz and C4. This finding has also been previously reported by other groups (Coch et al., 2002;
Dykman et al., 2000). Dykman et al. (2000) compared the performance of a failure to thrive group (FTT)1 with controls during a lexical decision task. Dykman et al. (2000) as well as Coch et al. (2002) observed the dual-peak negative waveform. According to their explanation, the later peak may mirror a postlexical confidence. The two peaks of the N400 in both groups may have shown the postlexical revision necessary to match or mismatch the target.
1
FTT was a group of children with abnormally low weight for age and gender and some studies have shown that the FTT have poorer language development and less developed reading skills. J. Silva-Pereyra et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 49 (2003) 187–199

A P300 amplitude effect was expected during both F and W tasks given the different presentation probabilities of animals and non-animals. PRs displayed larger P300 amplitudes for animals compared to non-animals during the F task, as expected, but not during the W task. The non-significant
P300 effect during the W task in PRs can be interpreted as a less efficient categorization processing of two types of stimuli. Previous studies on dyslexic, reading disabled and y or subjects with attention deficit disorders have also reported an attenuation of the P300 effect (Diniz et al., 1997) and attributed it to less efficient processing systems. A prolonged P300 peak latency in the W task was also found in PRs as previous studies have shown (Silva-Pereyra et al., 2001). Latency of the P300 seems to reflect stimulus classification speed (Magliero et al., 1984) and it has also been commonly interpreted as reflecting the time needed for stimulus evaluation and memory updating (Polich and Kok, 1995). According to the so-called
‘memory updating model’ (Donchin and Coles,
1988), infrequent events elicit large P300 because the immediate memory for the preceding target stimulus has decayed and is refurbished by the neural events which occur upon the presentation of a new target stimulus. In contrast, frequent stimulus events, maintaining stronger representations, do not require as much updating and therefore yield smaller P300 waves. Our results support the notion that PRs take more time than controls to update their context model during the W task, because their processing system is not fast enough to read the odd trial and accommodate the new information in the previous memory model. The word categorization task we used needs both word recognition and semantic processes to allow a decision on whether an item belongs to one or other semantic category. The developing ability to read quickly, accurately and effortlessly is critical to skillful reading comprehension (Adams, 1990), because it frees cognitive resources to allow for the integration of a word and their context, which leads to understanding the meaning of a text
(Stanovich, 1980).
Taking together our behavioral and ERP results,
PRs may appear as underachievers in semantic tasks but not because of a semantic processing

197

deficit itself (Waterman and Lewandowski, 1994).
Our study suggests that PRs have deficiencies very early in the processing of words. Every word presented is a problem for these children because it takes much time and effort to be read, although when finally read, they can decide the semantic category to which the word belongs. PRs may use a less efficient and demanding strategy to process words. This strategy consumes extra attentional resources and seems to be slow, with the subject storing and accommodating new difficult information into a memory model before the next trial arrives. We suggest that the most invasive deficit that PRs face when reading takes place very early, probably at the first encounter with the processing of graphemes. In this way a chain reaction of general slowness is triggered. Thus, previous behavioral studies may have mistakenly concluded that the deficit is at semantic level. Our results challenge this interpretation.
Acknowledgments
This project was partially supported by Grant
IN209998 from DGAPA and J30964-H from
CONACyT. The authors acknowledge Miguel
´
Rodrıguez Espinosa for technical support.
References
Adams, M.J., 1990. Beginning to Read: Thinking and Learning about Print. MIT press, Cambridge.
Barret, S.E., Rugg, M.D., 1990. Event-related potentials and the semantic matching of pictures. Brain Cogn. 14, 201–212.
Beitchman, J.H., Young, A.R., 1997. Learning disorders with a special emphasis on reading disorders: a review of the past 10 years. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 36,
1020–1032.
Ben-Dror, I., Bentin, S., Frost, R., 1995. Semantic, phonologic and morphologic skills in reading disabled and normal children: evidence from perception and production of spoken
Hebrew. Read. Res. Quart. 30, 876–893.
Bentin, S., McCarthy, G., Wood, C.C., 1985. Event related potentials, lexical decision and semantic priming. Electroenceph. Clin. Nuerophysiol. 60, 343–355.
Bernal, J., Harmony, T., Rodriguez, M., et al., 2000. Auditory event related potentials in poor readers. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 36, 11–23.
Byrne, B., 1981. Deficient syntactic control in poor readers: is a weak phonetic memory code responsible? Appl. Psycholinguist. 2, 201–212.

198

J. Silva-Pereyra et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 49 (2003) 187–199

Champion, A., 1997. Knowledge of suffixed words: a comparison of reading disabled and nondisabled readers. Ann.
Dyslexia 47, 29–55.
Chwilla, D., Brown, C., Hagoort, P., 1995. The N400 as a function of the level processing. Psychophysiology 32,
274–285.
Coch, D., Maron, L., Wolf, M., Holcomb, P.J., 2002. Word and picture processing in children: an event related potential study. Dev. Neuropsychol. 22, 373–406.
Deacon, D., Hewitt, S., Yang, C.M., Nagata, M., 2000. Eventrelated potential indices of semantic priming using masked and unmasked words: evidence that the N400 does not reflect a post-lexical process. Cogn. Brain Res. 9, 137–146.
Dykman, R.A., Akerman, P.T., Loizou, P.C., Casey, P.H., 2000.
An event-related potential study of older children with an early history of failure to thrive. Dev. Neuropsychol. 18,
187–212.
De Jong, P.F., 1998. Working memory deficits of reading disabled children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 70, 75–96.
Diniz Jr., J., Mangabeira-Albernaz, P.L., Munhoz, M.S., Fukuda, Y., 1997. Cognitive potentials in children with learning disabilities. Acta Otolaryngol. (Stockh) 117, 211–213.
Donchin, E., Coles, M.G., 1988. Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? BBS 11, 355–425.
Friedman, D., Putnam, L., Sutton, S., 1990. Longitudinal and cross-sectional comparisons of young children’s cognitive
ERPs and behavior in a picture-matching task: preliminary findings. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 8, 213–321.
Gillon, G., Dodd, B., 1994. A prospective study of relationship between phonological, semantic and syntactic skills and specific reading disability. Read. Writ. 6, 321–345.
Halgren, E., 1990. Insights from evoked potentials into the neuropsychological mechanisms of reading. In: Scheibel,
A.B., Wechsler, A.F. (Eds.), Neurobiology of Higher Cognitive Function. Guilford Press, New York, pp. 103–150.
Hill, H., Strube, M., Roesch-Ely, D., Weisbrod, M., 2002.
Automatic versus controlled processes in semantic priming differentiation by event-related potentials. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 44, 197–218.
Holcomb, P.J., Ackerman, P.T., Dykman, R.A., 1985. Cognitive event-related potentials in children with attention and reading deficits. Psychophysiology 22, 656–667.
Holcomb, P.J., Ackerman, P.T., Dyckman, R.A., 1986. Auditory event-related potentials in attention and reading disabled boys. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 3, 263–273.
Holcomb, P.J., Coffey, S.A., Neville, H., 1992. Visual and auditory sentence processing: a developmental analysis using event-related brain potentials. Dev. Neuropsychol. 8,
203–241.
Holcomb, P.J., McPherson, W.B., 1994. Event-related brain potentials reflect semantic priming in an object decision task. Brain Cogn. 24, 259–276.
Iragui, V.J., Kutas, K., Mitchiner, M.R., Hillyard, S.A., 1993.
Effects of aging on event-related brain potentials and reaction times in an auditory oddball task. Psychophysiology
30, 10–22.

Isaki, E., Plante, E.J., 1997. Short-term and working memory differences in languageylearning disabled and normal adults.
Commun. Disord. 30, 427–436.
Jared, D., Levy, B.A., Rayner, K., 1999. The role of phonology in the activation of word meanings during reading: evidence from proofreading and eye movements. J. Exp. Psychol.
Gen. 128, 219–264.
Johnson Jr., R., 1989. Developmental evidence for modality dependent P300 generators: a normative study. Psychophysiology 26, 651–667.
Juottonen, K., Rebonsuo, A., Lang, H., 1996. Dissimilar age influences on two ERP waveforms (LPC and N400) reflecting semantic context effect. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 4,
99–107.
Khan, S.C., Frisk, V., Taylor, M., 1999. Neurophysiological measures of reading difficulty in very-low-birthweight children. Psychophysiology 36, 76–85.
Kutas, M., Hillyard, S., 1980. Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruently. Science 207,
203–204.
Kutas, M., van Petten, C.K., 1994. Psycholinguistics electrified. In: Gernsbacher, M.A. (Ed.), Handbook of Psycholinguistics. Academic Press, New York, pp. 83–143.
Luck, S.J., Hillyard, S.A., 1994. Electrophysiological correlates of feature detection during visual search. Psychophysiology
31, 291–308.
Magliero, A., Bashore, T.R., Coles, M., Donchin, E., 1984. On the dependence of P300 latency on stimulus evaluation processes. Psychophysiology 21, 171–186.
Neville, H., Coffey, S.A., Holcomb, P., Tallal, P., 1993. The neurobiology of sensory and language processing in language-impaired children. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 5, 235–253.
Nielsen-Bohlman, L., Knight, R., 1994. Electrophysiological dissociation of rapid memory mechanisms in humans.
NeuroReport 5, 1517–1521.
Nobre, A.C., McCarthy, G., 1994. Language-related ERPs: scalp distributions and modulation by word type and semantic priming. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 6, 233–255.
Polich, J., 1985. Semantic categorization and event-related potentials. Brain Lang. 26, 304–321.
Polich, J., Kok, A., 1995. Cognitive and biological determinants of P300: an integrative review. Biol. Psychol. 41,
103–146.
Rayner, K., Pollatsek, A., 1989. The Psychology of Reading.
Prentice Hall Englewood Cliffs, NJ, pp. 529.
´
Reigosa, V., Perez, M.C., Manzano, M., Antelo, J.M., 1994.
Sistema automatizado para explorar la lectura en escolares de habla hispana. Revista Latina de Pensamiento Lenguaje
2, 141–160.
Segalowitz, S.J., Wagner, W.J., Menna, R., 1992. Lateral versus frontal ERP predictors of reading skill. Brain Cogn. 20,
85–103.
´
´
Silva-Pereyra, J., Fernandez, T., Dıaz-Comas, L., et al., 2001.
Delayed P300 during color discrimination and Stenberg tasks in poor readers. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 40, 17–32.

J. Silva-Pereyra et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 49 (2003) 187–199
Snodgrass, J.G., McCullough, B., 1986. The role of visual similarity in picture categorization. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum.
Learn Mem. 12, 147–154.
Snodgrass, J.G., Vanderwart, M.A., 1980. A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for the name agreement, image agreement, familiarity and visual complexity. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Learn Mem. 6, 174–215.
Stauder, J.E.A., Molenaar, P.C.M., van der Molen, M.W., 1993.
Scalp topography of event-related brain potential sand cognitive transition during childhood. Child Dev. 64, 769–788.
Stanovich, K.E., 1980. Toward an interactive-compensatory model of individual differences in the development of reading fluency. Read. Res. Quart. 16, 32–71.
Stelmack, R.M., Saxe, B.J., Noldy-Cullum, N., Campbell,
K.B., Armitage, R., 1988. Recognition memory for words and event-related potentials: a comparison of normal and disabled readers. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 10, 185–200.
Swanson, H.L., 1992. Generality and modifiability of working memory among skilled and less killed readers. J. Educ.
Psychol. 84, 473–488.
Swanson, H.L., Sachse, L.C., 2001. A subgroup analysis of working memory in children with reading disabilities: domain-general or domain specific deficiency? J. Learn
Disabil. 34, 249–263.
Tallal, P., Miller, S., Fich, H., 1993. Neurobiological basis of speech. A case of the preeminence of temporal processing.
´
In: Tallal, P., Galaburda, A., Lllinas, R., Von Euler, C.
(Eds.), Temporal information processing in the nervous system: special reference to dyslexia and dysphasia, vol.,
682, New York, Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. pp. 27–47.

199

Taylor, M.J., Keenan, N.K., 1990. Event-related potentials to visual and language stimuli in normal and dyslexic children.
Psychophysiology 27, 318–327.
Taylor, M.J., Khan, S.C., 2000. Top-down modulation of early selective attention processes in children. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 37, 135–147.
Taylor, M.J., Smith, M.L., 1995. Maturational changes in the
ERPs to verbal and nonverbal memory tasks. J. Psychophysiol. 9, 283–297.
Vellutino, F.R., Scanlon, D.M., 1985. Free recall of concrete and abstract words in poor and normal readers. J. Exp.
Child Psychol. 39, 363–380.
Vellutino, F.R., Scanlon, D.M., Spearing, D., 1995. Semantic and phonological coding in poor and normal readers. J. Exp.
Child Psychol. 59, 76–123.
Vellutino, F.R., Scanlon, D.M., Tanzman, M.S., 1988. Lexical memory in poor and normal readers. Can. J. Psychol. 42,
216–242.
Wagner, R., Torgesen, J.K., 1987. The nature of phonological processing and its casual role in the acquisition of reading skill. Psychol. Bull. 101, 192–212.
Waterman, B., Lewandowski, L., 1993. Phonological and semantic processing in reading-disabled and nondisabled males at two age levels. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 55, 87–103.
Waterman, B., Lewandowski, L., 1994. Orthographic, phonologic and semantic processing in reading-disabled and nondisabled subjects. Percept. Motor Skills 79, 35–45.
Zecker, S.G., Zinner, T.E., 1987. Semantic code deficit for reading disabled children on auditory lexical decision task.
J. Reading Behav. 19, 177–189.

Similar Documents

Premium Essay

Thesis for College

...fi/bitstream/handle/.../Thesis%20Timo%20Aho.pdf?... by T Aho - ‎2012 - ‎Related articles The purpose of this thesis project was to find and create a better solution for handling ... for example, of names, preferred shoe sizes and address information. During this project, it was decided that a customer information system will to be cre-. Thesis Proposal For Management Information Systems Free ... www.termpaperwarehouse.com/.../thesis...management-information-syste... Free Essays on Thesis Proposal For Management Information Systems for students. Use our papers to help you with yours 1 - 20. [PDF]Web-based Information System for Land Management www.ucalgary.ca/engo_webdocs/MR/05.20223.LimanMao.pdf by L Mao - ‎2005 - ‎Cited by 1 - ‎Related articles Web-based Information System for Land Management .... 1.5 THESIS STRUCTURE. ..... Figure 4.7: Sample of Attribute Tables of Web-GIS Prototype System . [PDF]Developing effective hospital management information ... ro.ecu.edu.au/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=2411&context=theses by C Bain - ‎2014 - ‎Related articles Oct 5, 2014 - The central contention of this thesis is that the current ecosystem models in the information ... This research seeks to highlight an example of ... hospital management information system environment, using the technology. [PDF]Thesis Management System for Industrial Partner ... - IS MU is.muni.cz/th/374278/fi_b/thesis-text.pdf by V Dedík - ‎Related articles Keywords. Thesis, Thesis Management, Information...

Words: 525 - Pages: 3

Premium Essay

Thesis Writing

...------------------------------------------------- Thesis Writing: A Guide for Students By Jennifer Swenson The Sparrow’s introduction to thesis writing is a clear-cut and comprehensive tool for those who are about to embark on one of the more difficult projects in all of academia. Thesis writing is not an art; rather, it is the product of many months of research and painstaking hard work. Whether you are writing a master’s thesis, a PhD thesis, or any other form of this venerable genre, I hope this guide will serve you well. Thesis Writing Background What is a thesis? A thesis is essentially a research report. It addresses a very specific issue and describes what is known about that issue, what work the student has done to investigate or resolve it, and how that issue may play out in the future. It is the thesis writer’s responsibility to familiarize her with the history of the issue and the different points of view that exist. The thesis writer works with a mentor who is an expert in the field that the thesis concerns, but not necessarily an expert on that exact topic. Usually thesis topics are so specific that very few people in the world except the thesis writer herself could be considered an expert on them. Your thesis writing will make a contribution to the field about which you are writing, and in a larger sense, to all of human knowledge. A thesis is distinctively different from an undergraduate research report because it is so original. How Specific Should My Thesis Get? When writing a thesis, you should...

Words: 2925 - Pages: 12

Premium Essay

Thesis Writing

...------------------------------------------------- Thesis Writing: A Guide for Students By Jennifer Swenson The Sparrow’s introduction to thesis writing is a clear-cut and comprehensive tool for those who are about to embark on one of the more difficult projects in all of academia. Thesis writing is not an art; rather, it is the product of many months of research and painstaking hard work. Whether you are writing a master’s thesis, a PhD thesis, or any other form of this venerable genre, I hope this guide will serve you well. Thesis Writing Background What is a thesis? A thesis is essentially a research report. It addresses a very specific issue and describes what is known about that issue, what work the student has done to investigate or resolve it, and how that issue may play out in the future. It is the thesis writer’s responsibility to familiarize her with the history of the issue and the different points of view that exist. The thesis writer works with a mentor who is an expert in the field that the thesis concerns, but not necessarily an expert on that exact topic. Usually thesis topics are so specific that very few people in the world except the thesis writer herself could be considered an expert on them. Your thesis writing will make a contribution to the field about which you are writing, and in a larger sense, to all of human knowledge. A thesis is distinctively different from an undergraduate research report because it is so original. How Specific Should My Thesis Get? When writing a thesis, you should...

Words: 2925 - Pages: 12

Free Essay

Thesis Guide

...pr pr acti od ca uc l a ing sp a ects th es of is at un sw po th stg es rad is gu uate ide PRACTICAL ASPECTS OF PRODUSING A THESIS AT THE UNIVERSITY OF NEW SOUTH WALES P.GRADUATE A thesis submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Postgraduate Board January 2002 University of New South Wales Please note: the web version does not contain two sections of the printed version. The differences are due to differing formats which makes it impossible to convert some pages into a PDF format. Missing are a mock up of a UNSW Thesis/Project Report Sheet and the information in Appendix IV. A copy of the printed guide can be sent to you if you email your address to campaigns@unsw.edu.au. This missing information was taken from the Thesis Submission Pack which is available from New South Q on the Kensington campus (download from or phone: (02) 9385 3093). ABSTRACT This booklet is designed to assist research students with the practical aspects of producing a postgraduate research thesis at the University of New South Wales. As well as providing advice in regard to the University’s requirements, formatting, layout, referencing and the use of information technology, this guide also describes what some students might regard as the more arcane and ritualistic aspects of producing a PhD thesis, in particular, those associated with accepted academic conventions. A section on posture and ergonomics has also been included to help you...

Words: 12383 - Pages: 50

Free Essay

Thesis Statement

...Thesis Statement and Outline Online Shopping vs. Brick and Mortar Shopping Both forms of shopping have one goal in mind. That goal is to get the item that you desire. Many of the stores that you visit on a daily basis can come to you online. I. You can shop in the comfort of your own home. A. You do not have to worry about getting ready to go shopping. 1. You can shop in your pajamas if you prefer. 2. You can shop when it is convenient for you. B. You do not have to fight crowds in the mall or store. 1. There is no traffic to worry about getting to the stores. 2. During the holiday season, you do not have to worry about many people crowding you. II. You can see what you are buying. A. Depending on what you buy, you can feel the item and actually see the item. 1. You can feel the texture or weight of an item. 2. You can see if the item is big or overweight. B. There is no wondering if you are getting the item that you ordered. 1. You can be positive that you are getting the correct item. 2. You can see the exact shade or style of what you are buying in person. III. Instant gratification A. There is no waiting to receive your item. 1. You are able to take your purchase home that same day. 2. Site to store is an option with many stores and it is possible to pick it up that day. B. Being able to choose...

Words: 339 - Pages: 2

Free Essay

General Thesis

...INFORMATION AND COMMUNICATION TECHNOLOGY THESIS TITLE A PROJECT Presented to the Department of Information and Communication Technology, Garden City University College in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Bachelor of Science In Computer Science By NAME1 NAME2 Month, Year DECLARATION I hereby declare that the entire thesis work entitled, “……………..” submitted to the department of Information and Communication Technology, Garden City University College, in fulfilment of the requirement for the award of the degree of BSc Computer Science, is a bonafide record of my own work carried out under the supervision of Mr/Mrs/Ms . ……….. I further declare that the thesis either in part or full, has not been submitted earlier by me or others for the award of any degree in any University. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This section contains expressions of gratitude to advisor(s) and anyone who helped you:  1. technically (including materials, supplies) 2. intellectually (assistance, advice) 3. financially (for example, departmental support, travel grants)  ABSTRACT The abstract is an important component of your thesis. Presented at the beginning of the thesis, it is likely the first substantive description of your work read by an external examiner. The abstract is the last section to write. An abstract is not merely an introduction in the sense of a preface, preamble, or advance organizer that prepares the reader for the thesis. In addition to that function, it must...

Words: 2039 - Pages: 9

Free Essay

Thesis Statement

...The thesis statement that I have come up with for my “big idea” topic is: I believe we can better our economy by changing the way the government assists the citizens financially with programs such as food stamps, otherwise known as EBT. The government shapes society and the government needs to help society help themselves by making a few changes to the way it disburses our tax money. I think this thesis is going to be effective because it shows my main focus of the essay I will be writing, which is to change the way government assists families of low income. I want to stress the option of giving more money as school grants rather than giving it for food. Another option would also be limiting the options of foods that are okay to purchase with EBT. Right now, there are no limitation other than alcohol or pre-prepared foods. You can even purchase energy drinks at the moment. If people weren’t given everything for doing nothing, they may be more likely to further their education and get a better job to provide such luxuries as fatty foods or sweets and energy drinks. I see a major problem supporting this thesis with fallacy, mainly because I have such strong personal opinions. I am going to have to force myself to rely on straight facts and pure research to get my point across. For my research, I am going to stray away from any sort of blogs or websites put up as a riot against the government. I will do my best to find websites that are “.org” or .gov”. I think a good...

Words: 727 - Pages: 3

Free Essay

Hot to Write Thesis

...How to write chapter 1 of a Thesis: Basic Guide How to writer chapter 1 of the thesis? This is the mainly question on every researcher. In every thesis writing, some of the people say that the first part will be the most difficult part. Because here you must think of a topic that you can proposed and in this chapter you must conceptualize your whole thesis or your whole research. The whole research will be reflected by the first chapter. Some of the school have different format than other school so please use this guide for your references. Be sure to check out the Attributes of a Good Thesis before you start and check out the basic parts of the thesis also. This can also serve as your guide for your case study, research paper, and term paper. This will help you to understand the chapter 1 of your school paper works. Chapter 1: Introduction also includes the following: * Introduction This must include introduction of your study. You must tackle the field of your study.  Your introduction must be consisting of 1-2 pages only. * Background of the Study This must include some of the past study that is currently connected to your topic or study. You can include some of the history but it must be 2-3 lines only. * Rationale This section must describe the problem situation considering different forces such as global, national and local forces.  Stating some the existence of the problem included in your topic. * Objectives of the study The objective of your study...

Words: 533 - Pages: 3

Free Essay

Human Trafficking Facts, Statistics, Truth, Research Papers, Reports, Essays, Articles, Thesis, Dissertation

... Research papers, reports, essays, articles, thesis, dissertationHuman Trafficking Facts, Statistics, Truth, Research papers, reports, essays, articles, thesis, dissertationHuman Trafficking Facts, Statistics, Truth, Research papers, reports, essays, articles, thesis, dissertationHuman Trafficking Facts, Statistics, Truth, Research papers, reports, essays, articles, thesis, dissertationHuman Trafficking Facts, Statistics, Truth, Research papers, reports, essays, articles, thesis, dissertationHuman Trafficking Facts, Statistics, Truth, Research papers, reports, essays, articles, thesis, dissertationHuman Trafficking Facts, Statistics, Truth, Research papers, reports, essays, articles, thesis, dissertationHuman Trafficking Facts, Statistics, Truth, Research papers, reports, essays, articles, thesis, dissertationHuman Trafficking Facts, Statistics, Truth, Research papers, reports, essays, articles, thesis, dissertationHuman Trafficking Facts, Statistics, Truth, Research papers, reports, essays, articles, thesis, dissertationHuman Trafficking Facts, Statistics, Truth, Research papers, reports, essays, articles, thesis, dissertationHuman Trafficking Facts, Statistics, Truth, Research papers, reports, essays, articles, thesis, dissertationHuman Trafficking Facts, Statistics, Truth, Research papers, reports, essays, articles, thesis, dissertationHuman Trafficking Facts, Statistics, Truth, Research papers, reports, essays, articles, thesis, dissertationHuman Trafficking Facts, Statistics...

Words: 353 - Pages: 2

Free Essay

Mis Thesis Thesis Thesis

...thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis thesis...

Words: 595 - Pages: 3

Free Essay

Us History

...Worksheet Overall Thesis Statement (this will be the last sentence of your introduction and should contain the three main organizing points in your essay – for this essay it will likely be political, economic, social): I. Thesis of the first paragraph of the body (Political): 1. First piece of specific historical evidence that supports your thesis: a. Explanation of how this evidence supports your thesis: 2. Second piece of specific historical evidence that supports your thesis: a. Explanation of how this evidence supports your thesis: 3. Third piece of specific historical evidence that supports your thesis: a. Explanation of how this evidence supports your thesis: II. Thesis of the second paragraph of the body (Economic): 1. First piece of specific historical evidence that supports your thesis: a. Explanation of how this evidence supports your thesis: 2. Second piece of specific historical evidence that supports your thesis: a. Explanation of how this evidence supports your thesis: 3. Third piece of specific historical evidence that supports your thesis: a. Explanation of how this evidence supports your thesis: III. Thesis of the third paragraph of the body (Social): 1. First piece of specific historical evidence that supports your thesis: a. Explanation of how this evidence supports your thesis: 2. Second piece of specific historical evidence that supports your thesis: a. Explanation of how this evidence supports your thesis: 3. Third piece of...

Words: 270 - Pages: 2

Free Essay

Apple Paper

...highlighting tool in your word processing software. In addition, please have them identify and copy and paste your thesis statement and the topic sentences for each paragraph. |  |Exemplary |Proficient |Emerging |Not Yet Demonstrated | | |100% |86% |73% |60% | |INTRODUCTION / THESIS |Well-developed introduction |Introduction creates interest |Introduction adequately |Background details are a | | |engages the reader and creates |and contains background |explains the background of the|random collection of | |Background/History |interest. Contains detailed |information. Thesis clearly |problem, but may lack |information, are unclear, and | |Defining the Problem |background information and a |states a problem and the |clarity.  Thesis states a |may be loosely related to the | |Thesis Statement |clear explanation of the problem.|writer’s position is evident. |problem, but writer’s position|topic. Thesis/position is | | |Thesis clearly states a | |may not be evident. |vague or not stated. ...

Words: 1013 - Pages: 5

Premium Essay

Jnoo

...THESIS MANUAL INSTRUCTIONS CONCERNING THE PREPARATION OF THESES AND DISSERTATIONS Research and Graduate Studies Texas A&M University-Kingsville Kingsville, Texas 78363 (361) 593-2808 SPRING 2011 COPYRIGHT PRIVILEGES BELONG TO RESEARCH AND GRADUATE STUDIES Reproduction of this THESIS MANUAL requires the written permission of the Graduate Dean. FOREWORD The nature of a research study should be one in which the investigation leads to new knowledge or enhancement of existing knowledge in the student's field of study, either through acquisition of new data or re-examination and interpretation of existing data. At the graduate level, all students should learn how new knowledge is created, how experimentation and discovery are carried out, and how to think, act and perform independently in their discipline. Depending upon the degree to which the discipline has an applied orientation, the student can demonstrate mastery of the discipline through means such as research papers, literature reviews, artistic performances, oral/written presentations or case studies. The doctoral dissertation is viewed in academia as the ultimate model of documentation of the student's research. The characteristics of dissertation research include the theoretical background, description of the problem, the method which was used to solve the problem, interpretation of results and explanation of their significance. The student is expected to produce a product of excellent quality which reflects...

Words: 10528 - Pages: 43

Free Essay

Talambuhay Na Palahad

...CENTRAL EUROPEAN UNIVERSITY Thesis Writing and ETD Submission Guidelines for CEU MA/MSc Theses and PhD Dissertations (Revised and adopted by the CEU Senate 7 December 2007) The thesis or dissertation is the single most important element of a research degree. It is a test of the student’s ability to undertake and complete a sustained piece of independent research and analysis, and to write up that research in a coherent form according to the rules and conventions of the academic community. As the official language of study at CEU is English, students are required to write the thesis/dissertation in English to a standard that native speaker academics would find acceptable. A satisfactory thesis should not only be adequate in its methodology, in its analysis and in its argument, and adequately demonstrate its author’s familiarity with the relevant literature; it should also be written in correct, coherent language, in an appropriate style, correctly following the conventions of citation. It should, moreover, have a logical and visible structure and development that should at all times assist the reader’s understanding of the argument being presented and not obscure it. The layout and physical appearance of the thesis should also conform to university standards. The purpose of this document is to outline the standard requirements and guidelines that a master’s thesis or PhD dissertation (hereafter the term ‘thesis’ is used to cover both MA and PhD except where the PhD...

Words: 5926 - Pages: 24

Free Essay

Just Like a River

...English speaking audiences. However, with this translation, the book can show any reader despite their beliefs can relate to the complexities of all relationships when people are unable to be open and share their feelings a learned behavior from society, family, or religious beliefs. See if it this meets the requirement thus far. Instructions Below: Your introduction must be no more than one paragraph in length. It should indicate the theme(s) and thesis/theses of the book, and you should include your thesis statement at the end of the introductory paragraph. The thesis statement is ABSOLUTELY essential to your paper. It tells me what your analyses will prove or argue. Your thesis statement should be an argument about the author’s purpose in writing the book or the author’s thesis in the book - and how successful (or not) was the author in achieving this purpose or proving this thesis. This may seem a bit confusing, but think of your thesis statement creation as a three step process. * First, identify what you think is the thesis or purpose of the book. *...

Words: 626 - Pages: 3