Free Essay

Business

In:

Submitted By kalebmicah1
Words 10519
Pages 43
Arch Sex Behav (2011) 40:699–712 DOI 10.1007/s10508-010-9671-7

ORIGINAL PAPER

Physiological and Subjective Sexual Arousal in Self-Identified Asexual Women
Lori A. Brotto • Morag A. Yule

Received: 27 October 2009 / Revised: 27 July 2010 / Accepted: 27 July 2010 / Published online: 21 September 2010 Ó Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010

Abstract Asexuality can be defined as a lifelong lack of sexual attraction. Empirical research on asexuality reveals significantly lower self-reported sexual desire and arousal and lower rates of sexual activity; however, the speculation that there may also be an impaired psychophysiological sexual arousal response has never been tested. The aim of this study was to compare genital (vaginal pulse amplitude; VPA) and subjective sexual arousal in asexual and non-asexual women. Thirty-eight women between the ages of 19 and 55 years (10 heterosexual, 10 bisexual, 11 homosexual, and 7 asexual) viewed neutral and erotic audiovisual stimuli while VPA and self-reported sexual arousal and affect were measured. There were no significant group differences in the increased VPA and self-reported sexual arousal response to the erotic film between the groups. Asexuals showed significantly less positive affect, sensuality-sexual attraction, and self-reported autonomic arousal to the erotic film compared to the other groups; however, there were no group differences in negative affect or anxiety. Genital-subjective sexual arousal concordance was significantly positive for the asexual women and nonsignificant for the other three groups, suggesting higher levels

of interoceptive awareness among asexuals. Taken together, the findings suggest normal subjective and physiological sexual arousal capacity in asexual women and challenge the view that asexuality should be characterized as a sexual dysfunction. Keywords Asexuality Á Genital arousal Á Vaginal pulse amplitude Á Hypoactive sexual desire disorder Á Sexual attraction

Introduction Human asexuality is, loosely defined, as the absence of sexual attraction. In the last several years, there have been a number of empirical examinations focused on the description of asexuality and the characterization of asexual individuals. The only existing study to explore the prevalence of asexuality found that approximately 1% of the population report a lack of sexual attraction (Bogaert, 2004) and therefore likely identify as ‘‘asexual.’’Others who have studied asexuality have employed differing definitions, including: a lack of sexual behavior (Rothblum & Brehony, 1993), a lack of sexual orientation (Storms, 1980), and a lack of sexual desire or excitement (Prause & Graham, 2007). With the emergence of Google, this allowed individuals who previously had great difficulty finding and interacting with other asexuals online to easily locate one another and develop an online group where questions and answers could be posed, and experiences could be shared and explored. The largest online web-community of asexuals, known as the Asexuality Visibility and Education Network (AVEN), was founded in 2001 by Jay, and the group came to define themselves another way—focusing on the lack of sexual attraction but adding that ‘‘every asexual experiences sexual desire and arousal somewhat differently’’ (Jay, 2003).

L. A. Brotto Department of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada M. A. Yule Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada L. A. Brotto (&) Division of Gynaecologic Oncology, University of British Columbia, 2775 Laurel St., 6th Floor, Vancouver, BC V5Z 1M9, Canada e-mail: lori.brotto@vch.ca

123

700

Arch Sex Behav (2011) 40:699–712

The development of this ‘‘community,’’ led by AVEN’s founder, Jay, has facilitated the emergence of an online‘‘asexual identity’’ (Scherrer, 2008), as the structure of AVEN’s pages were specifically designed around ‘‘the process of identity formation’’ (Jay, 2003, p. 6). As the curious but not-yet-self-identified asexual navigated from ‘‘static pages’’ providing basic information and a definition of asexuality to the interactive‘‘web forum,’’ a collective identity of asexuality was thought to emerge (Jay, 2003; Scherrer, 2008). The labeling and identification of oneself as asexual is a major aim of AVEN and the asexuality movement, and likely represents something akin to the homosexuality identity movement of the 1950s and 1960s that was facilitated by the Mattachine society (Sears, 2006). More than a vehicle for facilitating identity formation, AVEN is also used to transform this collective identity into collective action, providing education to the greater public with the goal of lessening stigma associated with asexuality. There has been a recent frenzy of media attention on asexuality (e.g., 20/20, 2006; CNN Showbiz Tonight, 2006; Fox News Dayside, 2006; Montel Williams Show, 2007; The View, 2006; Tucker Carlson, 2006), some of which has suggested that asexual individuals experience a type of sexual dysfunction known as Hypoactive Sexual Desire Disorder (HSDD), and that this group may represent the polar lower end of the desire continuum (Montel Williams Show, 2007). The implications, if this assumption is correct, are significant given the current climate of sexual pharmaceuticals and increasing concentration on diagnosing, treating, and curing low sexual desire (Tiefer, 2002, 2006). Upon the asexual’s presentation to the sex therapy clinic, perhaps at the insistence of a distressed and dissatisfied partner, there may be a premature temptation on the part of the clinician to seek pharmaceutical and/or hormonal treatments for the lifelong lack of sexual interest. However, the existing data suggest that asexuals do not experience distress nor want to be ‘‘fixed’’ (Brotto, Knudson, Inskip, Rhodes, & Erskine, 2010; Pagan Westfall, 2004), making asexuality fundamentally different from HSDD. And although a subgroup of women with HSDD similarly report being satisfied with the prospect of never having sex again, what seems to separate this group from asexuals is that the latter report never having experienced desire or sexual attraction whereas the former may have experienced desire at some point but then lost it. In reality, the precise delineation between lifelong HSDD and asexuality is unclear, and this ‘‘gray zone’’ requires further study. In one qualitative study with four asexuals, Prause and Graham (2007) noted that sexual arousal, in addition to sexual desire, might also be impaired. One female participant expressed that a sexually explicit film would have no effect on her sexual arousal (though sexual arousal was not assessed in that study). In a follow-up quantitative phase, Prause and Graham therefore predicted that asexuals would report lower levels of sexual arousal compared to a group of non-asexuals

and that this might be due to a higher threshold for sexual arousal in the former. If this were true, it was hypothesized that the asexuals would show lower scores on a measure of sexual arousability. A low score on the Sexual Arousability Inventory (Hoon, Wincze, & Hoon, 1976) was the best predictor of membership in the asexual compared to the sexual group. Moreover, low scores on the dyadic sexual desire and the solitary sexual desire subscales of the Sexual Desire Inventory (Spector, Carey, & Steinberg, 1996) as well as the sexual excitation subscale of the Sexual Inhibition and Sexual Excitation Scales (Janssen, Vorst, Finn, & Bancroft, 2002) also significantly predicted asexuality group membership. Thus, in addition to Prause and Graham’s speculations about low desire best differentiating asexuals from sexuals, it was, in fact, measures of arousability that best distinguished the groups. Prause and Graham suggested that measuring sexual arousal with psychophysiological instruments might further elucidate if these observed group differences in arousal might also apply to asexuals’ sexual physiological responding. In a more recent study designed to explore sexual response and distress in asexuals, 187 asexual men and women completed a web-based questionnaire followed by in-depth telephone interviews with a subset of 15 asexuals (Brotto et al., 2010). Among those asexuals who had been sexually active in the past 4 weeks, women showed low arousal scores on the Female Sexual Function Index (Rosen et al., 2000) comparable to a larger validation group of women with Female Sexual Arousal Disorder (FSAD; Wiegel, Meston, & Rosen, 2005). However, asexual men showed normal levels of erectile functioning (the analogue of arousal in men) on the International Index of Erectile Functioning (Rosen et al., 1997), suggesting that if arousal was impaired, it might be a feature only of female and not of male asexuality. In the qualitative phase of this study, several asexuals spoke about their genitals in somewhat technical, emotionally bare language and most believed that they lacked a sexual arousal response (Brotto et al., 2010). Although this limited research proposes that genital sexual arousal in asexuals might be impaired (along with self-reported desire), to date, there have been no studies of asexuals’ psychophysiological sexual response to a competent sexual stimulus, and prior studies that have measured sexual arousal utilized a self-report questionnaire focused on retrospective recall over a fixed period of time. Thus, the extent to which asexuals’ self-reports of lack of sexual arousal reflect subjective only, physiological only, or both aspects of sexual responding is unknown. Sexual psychophysiological techniques have been used in research throughout the twentieth century, and more recently have been used in studies of sexual orientation. The field gained widespread interest following the work of Masters and Johnson (1966) who studied the sexual arousal patterns of their patients using a variety of physiological measures. In women, vaginal

123

Arch Sex Behav (2011) 40:699–712

701

photoplethysmography is one of several psychophysiological techniques (Sintchak & Geer, 1975). It involves a tamponshaped, usually acrylic, probe, which emits infrared light and provides an index of absorbed light corresponding to the degree of genital congestion. The study of vaginal photoplethysmography has led to some important advances in our understanding of women’s sexual response. For example, whereas men show patterns of genital arousal that correspond to their stated sexual orientation (Rieger, Chivers, & Bailey, 2005), lesbian and heterosexual women showed the same degree of increase in vaginal pulse amplitude (VPA; the more sensitive and specific index in vaginal photoplethysmography), regardless of their stated sexual orientation and irrespective of the stimuli shown—whether heterosexual, homosexual, or non-human primate (Chivers, Rieger, Latty, & Bailey, 2004; Chivers, Seto, & Blanchard, 2007). If such ‘‘target non-specificity’’ was a feature of all women, including asexuals, then one might predict that asexual women would show a similar degree of genital response to stimuli compared to heterosexual and lesbian women despite their stated preference for no sexual partners. Another reason why one might predict similar VPA responses in asexual women compared to other sexual orientation groups stems from the recent finding that the majority (77%) of asexual women had engaged in masturbation—the average frequency of which was once/month (Brotto et al., 2010), a result that replicates previous research indicating that asexuals did not differ significantly from non-asexuals in their desire to masturbate (Prause & Graham, 2007). Although this speculation is based on indirect reasoning, it is possible that because a high level of genital arousal is necessary for orgasm to occur (Basson et al., 2003; Masters & Johnson, 1966), and because orgasm is a positive reinforcer of masturbation (Herbenick et al., 2009), it may be that asexuals experience a normal level of genital arousal which, in part, provides incentive for their masturbatory activity. If, on the other hand, asexuality was simply one manifestation of a lifelong sexual dysfunction, as has been suggested by some (see review of this topic on the online asexuality blog by Pretzelboy, 2009), then one might hypothesize lower levels of sexual arousal while being tested in a controlled laboratory environment with potent sexual stimuli. There is some limited evidence for this assertion: In some select subgroups of women, those with self-reported genital arousal impairments were found to have significantly lower VPA compared to a sexually healthy comparison group (Brotto, Basson, & Gorzalka, 2004; Pieterse et al., 2008; Wincze, Albert, & Bansal, 1993), and women with HSDD were found to have different patterns of activation on functional magnetic resonance imaging compared to women without HSDD (Arnow et al., 2009). Thus, if asexuality was characterized by atypical sexual response in the form of lack of sexual excitement, then one might predict significantly lower levels of physiological arousal in asexual women compared to women of other sexual orientation groups.

The concordance between subjective and psychophysiological sexual arousal has been a very important and interesting aspect of study in the vaginal photoplethysmography literature. This research has consistently shown a low correlation between self-reported sexual arousal and VPA across numerous studies (as summarized in the meta-analysis by Chivers, Seto, Lalumiere, Laan, & Grimbos, 2010), and concludes that women show significantly less genital-subjective concordance than men. In their review, Chivers et al. predicted that concordance would be low when actual genital responding was low. It follows, therefore, that if VPA was low in asexual women, genital-subjective concordance might also be low due to inability to detect changes in blood flow (Heiman, 1977). The aim of the present study was to compare genital and subjective sexual arousal in a sample of asexual women with responses to women from other sexual orientation groups (heterosexual, homosexual, and bisexual women). We predicted lower levels of self-reported sexual arousal in the asexual women compared to the other groups. Regarding VPA, we predicted that if asexuality was characterized by atypical sexual responses, then we would find lower VPA in the asexuals compared to the other three groups, but we hypothesized no significant group differences in VPA if asexuality was not reflective of problematic sexual responding. We included a homosexual and bisexual sample of women based on the conclusions of Chivers et al. (2004, 2007) that women show a non-target specific pattern of genital responding. Thus, we expected that all groups of women would show the same pattern of VPA to the heterosexual stimuli. We also explored the degree of concordance between genital and subjective sexual arousal in each of these subgroups separately.

Method Participants A total of 38 women between the ages of 19 and 55 years participated in this study (10 heterosexual, 10 bisexual, 11 homosexual, and 7 asexual), and were assigned to sexual orientation group according to self-identification. Participants were recruited through several separate avenues, including postings on local websites (e.g., Craigslist), on the AVEN online web-community, through the university’s human subject pool, and through advertisements in the community. The wording of advertisements differed depending on which sexual orientation group was being targeted. Two separate ads were used to target asexuals. Both ads read, ‘‘Do you experience a lack of sexual attraction towards others?’’and‘‘Do you find it hard to relate when people talk about sexual attraction?’’ The first ad also included the text ‘‘The Sexual Health Lab is looking for female participants who identify as asexual,

123

702

Arch Sex Behav (2011) 40:699–712

straight, lesbian or bisexual to take part in a study on sexual orientation.’’ The second ad deliberately did not contain any reference to self-identification as asexual. Both ads further read ‘‘If you can relate to the statements above, please contact us for more information about this study.’’ The majority of asexual women (N = 6) were recruited via the AVEN website and one asexual woman was recruited via the second ad. Self-identification as asexual was further verified in person according to the free-response item on the demographic questionnaire that asked women to write their sexual orientation status. The average age of participants was 28.1 years for heterosexuals (SD = 3.8), 22.8 years for bisexuals (SD = 4.4), 36.3 years for homosexuals (SD = 11.1), and 33.6 for asexuals (SD = 9.9), and there was a significant group difference in age, F(3, 34) = 5.76, p\.01, with bisexual women being younger than the other groups. There were no significant group differences in highest level of education achieved, v2(3) = 5.58, p\.05, with the majority of participants (100% of homosexual, 90% of bisexual, 91% of homosexual, and 86% of asexual) having at least some post-secondary university education. Fifty percent of heterosexuals, 70% of bisexuals, 63% of homosexuals, and 28% of asexuals indicated that they were currently in a committed relationship, and these proportions differed significantly, v2(3) = 10.21, p\.05, with asexuals being least likely to be in a relationship. The majority (70–90% across groups) identified themselves as European/Canadian.

desire domain included items such as‘‘Over the past 4 weeks, how often did you feel sexual desire or interest?’’ Examples of items from the arousal domain included: ‘‘Over the past 4 weeks, how often did you feel sexually aroused (‘‘turned on’’) during sexual activity or intercourse?’’ and ‘‘Over the past 4 weeks, how confident were you about becoming sexually aroused during sexual activity or intercourse?’’ The total FSFI score was obtained by summing the scores from the individual domains, with higher scores on this measure indicating better levels of sexual function. If one domain subtotal could not be calculated (due to missing items), then neither could the FSFI total score. The FSFI has been validated as an appropriate tool to differentiate between women with and without FSAD (Wiegel et al., 2005), has a high degree of internal consistency (Cronbach’s a of 0.82 and higher), and high test–retest reliability (r = .79–.86). In light of some conceptual and statistical problems identified by Meyer-Bahlburg and Dolezal (2007), adjustments were made to the scoring of the FSFI in the current study in that any woman who had not engaged in sexual activity over the preceding month received ‘‘missing value codes’’for items inquiring about sexual response during sexual activity (as opposed to receiving a zero score, which erroneously inflates scores denoting dysfunction (Brotto, 2009)). Missing coded items were then excluded from the calculation of the FSFI total score and were not included in the calculation of the mean for that subscale across all participants. Detailed Assessment of Sexual Arousal (DASA)

Measures Demographic Information Participants were asked to complete demographic information on ethnicity, sex/gender, sexual orientation, occupation, annual income, education, presence of sexual concerns, relationship status, number of sexual and romantic partners, and masturbation frequency. All questions were in a free-response format given the finding that asexuals respond differently to forced choice versus free response formats in answering questions about gender and sexual orientation (Brotto et al., 2010; Prause & Graham, 2007), apart from questions pertaining to education, relationship status, and masturbation frequency, which had forced-choice response options. The DASA is an unpublished questionnaire that has been found to significantly differentiate aspects of sexual arousal in women with arousal difficulties (Basson & Brotto, 2003). Subscales include: mental excitement, genital tingling/throbbing, genital wetness, and pleasant genital sensations, with each subscale assessing level of arousal in response to a variety of different forms of sexual stimulation (e.g., from verbal/written stimuli to breast stimulation to various forms of genital stimulation). Items were rated on a 1–7 Likert scale from (1) low to (7) intense. Sexual Desire Inventory (SDI) The SDI (Spector et al., 1996) is a 15-item self-administered questionnaire intended to be a specific measure of sexual desire. Unlike the FSFI subscale of desire, which focuses on frequency and intensity of feeling desire, the SDI attempts to capture a cognitive (rather than behavioral) aspect of sexual desire, and produces a total SDI score as well as dyadic and solitary desire subscale scores. Individuals not in a relationship are able to receive a score on the dyadic subscale, as the items focus on sexual thoughts and desires involving a partner, and not sexual behavior, as illustrated with the following item on the SDI: ‘‘During the past month, how often have you had sexual thoughts involving a partner?’’ The SDI has been

Female Sexual Function Index (FSFI) The FSFI (Rosen et al., 2000) is a 19-item scale assessing six domains of sexual function: desire, subjective arousal, lubrication, orgasm, satisfaction, and pain during sexual activity over the past month. Items were rated on a 6-point scale, with lower scores indicating lower levels of sexual functioning, and a zero score indicating no sexual activity in the past month. The

123

Arch Sex Behav (2011) 40:699–712

703

validated with non-clinical populations of healthy women and men. Items were rated on an 8-point scale, with higher scores indicating better levels of sexual desire. Film Scale The Film Scale (Heiman & Rowland, 1983) is a 34-item selfreport questionnaire that assesses perception of genital sexual arousal (e.g., warmth in genitals, genital pulsing or throbbing), subjective sexual arousal (e.g., turned on), autonomic arousal (e.g., faster breathing, faster heart beat), anxiety, positive affect (e.g., pleasure, interested, excited), negative affect (e.g., worried, angry, disgusted), and sensuality-sexual attraction (e.g., sensuous, a desire to be close to someone, sexually attractive) in response to sexual stimuli. Items were rated on a 7-point Likert scale from (1) not at all to (7) intensely in which participants indicated how much they endorsed each item at the present moment. Psychophysiological Recording Genital arousal was measured using the vaginal photoplethysmograph and the VPA signal was the primary endpoint given that it is a sensitive and specific measure of genital arousal (Laan, Everaerd, & Evers, 1995). A single vaginal probe (Behavioral Technology Inc., Salt Lake City, UT) measured VPA, which was monitored throughout exposure to each neutral and erotic film segment. VPA was recorded on a personal computer (HP Pentium M Laptop), which collected, converted (from analog to digital, using a Model MP150WSW data acquisition unit [BIOPAC Systems, Inc.]), and transformed data, using the software program AcqKnowledge III, Version 3.8.1 (BIOPAC Systems, Inc., Santa Barbara, CA). The signal was band-pass filtered (0.5–30 Hz) and a sampling rate of 200 samples/s was used for VPA throughout the neutral and erotic film exposure. Artifact smoothing took place following visual inspection of the data by an investigator who was blind to the sexual orientation group of the participant, and data were subsequently analyzed in 30-s segments. For analyses of VPA, we compared the first three 30-s segments of the neutral condition to the last five 30-s segments of the erotic condition. The probe was disinfected in a solution of Cidex OPA (ortho-phthalaldehyde 0.55%), a high level disinfectant (Advanced Sterilization Products, Irvine, CA, USA), promptly following each session. Sexual Stimuli Participants viewed a 3-min neutral film (either a documentary about lei making or a travelogue about Hawaii) followed by an 8-min erotic film (a Candida Royalle, female-friendly film consisting of progressively increasing sexual intensity of heterosexual manual genital stimulation, oral sex, and penilevaginal intercourse clips which were spliced together and

joined by accompanying background music). A 1-min segment in which the word ‘‘Relax’’ was displayed on the television screen with no audio output preceded these films. Participants watched one of two film sets, each containing a neutral and erotic segment, and previous research has supported the similarity of women’s arousal to and ratings of these two film sets (Brotto, Basson, & Luria, 2008). Procedure Our university’s clinical research ethics board approved all procedures. Participants responding to community and online advertisements took part in a telephone screening process with one trained female research assistant in which the procedures of the study were described and any questions answered. They were told that they would be watching heterosexual films and that if this were objectionable to them, they could withdraw from the study. None withdrew following the telephone screening. The purpose of the screening was also to ensure that women in the‘‘probable asexual’’group did not meet criteria for HSDD. To do so, a series of questions based on the DSM-IV-TR criteria for HSDD (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) were asked by a trained interviewer. Although women may have met criterion A (i.e., absent/deficient desire for sexual activity), none met criterion B (i.e., marked distress). Women in the other sexual orientation groups were similarly asked if they experienced any sexual difficulties with the intent that any woman who did would be excluded; none met this exclusion criterion. Participants were tested from 2008 to 2009 in a psychophysiological research room located in the university hospital, which contained a comfortable reclining chair, two bookcases, a large screen TV, an intercom, and a sink with small cupboard unit. A thin blanket was placed over the seating area of the chair. Following written consent, participants were tested by a female researcher. Next, participants completed the questionnaire battery in the private psychophysiology room and were provided with further instructions about the vaginal photoplethysmograph and the remainder of the procedures. The researcher then left the room while participants inserted the probe and informed the researcher of their readiness via intercom. In order to habituate to the testing environment, participants were encouraged to relax on a comfortable reclining chair for a 10 min period after the probe was inserted but before watching the video segments. Subjective sexual arousal and affect were assessed at the end of the adaptation period using the Film Scale. Following the completion of the Film Scale, the video sequence began. The audio component was delivered via wireless headphones. Immediately after the video sequence, participants filled out a second Film Scale, which asked them to evaluate how they felt during the last (erotic) film. They were then instructed to remove the probe and meet the researcher in a separate room where they had their finger-lengths measured using a standard

123

704

Arch Sex Behav (2011) 40:699–712

computer scanner (data not presented here). Participants were debriefed and provided with either $20 remuneration or 2 course credits, depending on recruitment method. Statistical Analyses Baseline group comparisons used analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by the Tukey’s multiple comparisons test in cases of a significant overall effect. Given the significant group difference in age, we conducted a repeated measure ANOVA with age as the covariate (ANCOVA) for all sexual arousal endpoints. There was no significant Film x Age interaction, p[ .05. We thus did not covary for age in remaining analyses and instead conducted a series of Group 9 Film ANOVAs. Effect sizes for all repeated measures analyses were calculated with the partial eta-squared (g2).

scored significantly lower than every other group of women, with no significant between-group differences between the heterosexual, homosexual, or bisexual women (Table 1). Due to one participant not completing the SDI, information on desire for solitary and dyadic sex was based on six asexuals. On the SDI Dyadic subscale, the overall ANOVA was significant, F(3, 33) = 12.83, p\.001, with a Tukey’s test showing significantly lower dyadic desire in the asexuals compared to the heterosexual (p\.001), homosexual (p\.001), and bisexual (p\.001) women. The overall ANOVA was not significant on the SDI Solitary subscale, F(3, 33)\1, indicating no significant group differences in desire for solitary sexual activity (Table 1). During the neutral baseline period, there were no significant group differences on any measure of subjective sexual arousal, affect, or perceived autonomic arousal on the Film Scale, all ps[.05. Physiological Sexual Arousal There was a significant main effect of Film on VPA, F(7, 231) = 30.77, p\.001, partial g2 = 0.48, such that the erotic film increased VPA in all groups. There was no significant Group 9 Film interaction, F(21, 231)\1, partial g2 = 0.04 (Fig. 1). Self-Reported Sexual Arousal On subjective sexual arousal, there was a main effect of Film, F(1, 34) = 61.33, p\.001, partial g2 = 0.64, such that subjective arousal increased after the erotic film. There was no significant Group 9 Film interaction, F(3, 34)\1, partial g2 = 0.06 (Fig. 2A). On perceived physical sexual arousal, there was a significant main effect of Film, F(1, 34) = 101.34, p\.001, partial g2 = 0.75, such that the erotic film significantly increased reports of perceived genital arousal. There was also a marginally significant Group 9 Film interaction, F(3, 34) = 2.52, p = .075 which suggested that there was a more robust increase in perceived genital arousal in the nonasexual groups compared to the asexual group; however, the effect size was small, partial g2 = 0.18 (Fig. 2b). On the sensuality-sexual attraction domain of the Film Scale, there was a significant Group 9 Film interaction, F(3, 34) = 3.41, p = .028, partial g2 = 0.23, which revealed significant increases in sensuality-sexual attraction with exposure to the erotic film in the heterosexual, homosexual, and bisexual women but no significant change among the asexual women (Fig. 2c). Self-Reported Affect There was a significant Group 9 Film interaction on positive affect, F(3, 34) = 2.96, p = .046, partial g2 = 0.21, which showed that only the non-asexual groups experienced a significant

Results Measures of Sexual Function Due to incomplete data from one asexual, information on masturbation frequency was based on six asexuals. There were no significant group differences in masturbation frequency, F(3, 33)\1, with a mean frequency of between one to three times per month (Table 1), and these rates were comparable to those in a previous study of masturbation frequency in asexuals (Brotto et al., 2010). According to the FSFI, four (57%) out of seven asexuals had been sexually active, either alone or with a partner, in the past 4 weeks. The corresponding rates for heterosexual, bisexual, and lesbian women were: 90, 100, and 82%. There was a significant overall ANOVA effect on the FSFI Desire subscale, F(3, 37) = 8.67, p\.001. A Tukey’s test showed the asexual women to have significantly lower scores than the heterosexual (p = .001), bisexual (p\.001), and homosexual (p = .011) women, with none of the other groups significantly differing from one another. There were no significant overall ANOVA effects on FSFI Arousal, F(3, 31) = 1.88; FSFI Lubrication, F(3, 30)\1; FSFI Orgasm, F(3, 31)\ 1; FSFI Satisfaction, F(3, 25) = 1.41; FSFI Pain, F(3, 22)\1; or the FSFI Total Score, F(3, 21)\1 (Table 1). Scores on the FSFI subscales of Desire, Arousal, and Lubrication in the present study were also comparable to those of asexual women in previous research (Brotto et al., 2010); however, asexual women in the present study had slightly higher scores on the FSFI subscales of Orgasm and Pain and slightly lower scores on the Satisfaction subscale. On the DASA, there was a significant overall ANOVA for mental sexual arousal, F(3, 34) = 15.79, p\.001; genital tingling/throbbing, F(3, 34) = 12.44, p\.001; genital wetness, F(3, 33) = 4.19, p = .013; and pleasant genital sensations, F(3, 34) = 12.51, p\.001. For each of the four subscales, asexuals

123

Arch Sex Behav (2011) 40:699–712 Table 1 Sexual behavior measures as a function of group Measure Group Heterosexual (n = 10) Masturbationi Never Few times/year Once/month 2–3/month Once/week 2–4/week Once/day [Once/day 3 (30%) 1 (10%) 1 (10%) 2 (20%) 0 (0%) 2 (20%) 1 (10%) 0 (0%) M (SD) FSFI desirea FSFI arousalb FSFI lubricationb FSFI orgasmb FSFI satisfactionc,j FSFI painb,j FSFI total score DASA mental arousale DASA genital tingling/throbbinge DASA genital wetnesse DASA pleasant genital sensationse SDI dyadici,f SDI solitaryi,g d 705

p Bisexual (n = 10) Homosexual (n = 11) Asexual (n = 7)

1 (10%) 2 (20%) 0 (0%) 1 (10%) 0 (0%) 6 (60%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) M (SD) 4.38 (1.23) 4.77 (1.20) 5.22 (1.22) 4.76 (1.40) 4.67 (1.30) 5.30 (0.97) 28.21 (7.11) 5.29 (0.67) 5.27 (0.91) 4.56 (1.32) 5.64 (0.79) 46.20 (6.89) 11.80 (6.83)

2 (18.2%) 3 (27.3%) 1 (9.1%) 0 (0%) 3 (27.3%) 2 (18.2%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) M (SD) 3.38 (1.32) 4.67 (1.24) 5.27 (0.87) 4.80 (1.61) 5.20 (0.56) 5.20 (1.60) 28.47 (7.23) 4.63 (1.43) 4.72 (1.47) 4.61 (1.51) 4.71 (1.65) 38.27 (13.96) 10.45 (5.28)

2 (33.3%) 1 (16.7%) 1 (16.7%) 1 (16.7%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 1 (16.7%) 0 (0%) M (SD) 1.37 (0.29) 3.00 (1.82) 4.60 (1.14) 4.30 (1.58) –h –h –h 2.19 (0.76) 2.21 (0.81) 2.48 (1.43) 2.28 (1.41) 10.33 (5.16) 7.00 (6.20)

ns

3.90 (1.55) 4.87 (1.58) 4.90 (1.47) 5.02 (1.56) 4.67 (1.25) 5.45 (0.83) 28.61 (7.15) 5.56 (1.11) 5.23 (1.15) 4.86 (1.65) 5.96 (1.24) 39.30 (14.58) 11.70 (7.83)

\.01 ns ns ns ns ns ns \.001 \.001 \.01 \.001 \.001 ns

Absolute ranges: a 1.2–6, b 0–6, c 0.8–6, d 2–36, e 1–7, f 6–62, g 2–23; h not available; i based on 6 asexuals; j based on 1 asexual so data are not presented ns non-significant

1.00 0.80

0.60 0.40 0.20 neutral 0.00 30 60 90 300 330 360 390 420 erotic

increase in positive affect with the erotic film (Fig. 3a). On negative affect, there was neither a main effect of Film, F(1, 34)\1, partial g2 = 0.02, nor a significant Group 9 Film interaction, F(3, 34) = 1.71, partial g2 = 0.13 (Fig. 3b). Anxiety significantly decreased following exposure to the erotic film in all groups, F(1, 32) = 10.07, p = .003, partial g2 = 0.24, with no Group 9 Film interaction, F(3, 32)\1, partial g2 = 0.04 (Fig. 3c).

VPA (mV)

Self-Reported Autonomic Arousal There was a significant interaction between Group and Film on self-reported autonomic arousal, F(3, 34) = 5.06, p = .005, partial g2 = 0.31, as well as a main effect of erotic film, F(1, 34) = 40.08, p\.001, partial g2 = 0.54. Multiple comparison revealed a significant increase in autonomic arousal with the

Time (s) heterosexual bisexual homosexual asexual

Fig. 1 Effects of neutral and erotic stimuli on vaginal pulse amplitude (VPA) during the first 90 s of neutral and last 150 s of erotic film conditions. Error bars indicate standard error of measurement (SEM)

123

706 Fig. 2 Effects of neutral and erotic stimuli on a subjective sexual arousal, b self-reported physical sexual arousal, and c sensuality-sexual attraction in heterosexual, bisexual, homosexual, and asexual women. Error bars indicate SEM neutral erotic
5 5.5

Arch Sex Behav (2011) 40:699–712

a
Self-reported physical sexual arousal
4.4

b

4

Subjective sexual arousal

3

3.3

2

2.2

1

1.1

0 heterosexual bisexual homosexual asexual

0.0 heterosexual bisexual homosexual asexual

5

c

4

Sensuality/Sexual Attraction

3

2

1

0 heterosexual bisexual homosexual asexual

Fig. 3 Effects of neutral and erotic stimuli on a positive affect, b negative affect, c anxiety, and d autonomic arousal in heterosexual, bisexual, homosexual, and asexual women. Error bars indicate SEM

a
Positive affect

5.5 4.4 3.3 2.2 1.1 0

b neutral 2.0 1.6

Negative affect

erotic

1.2 0.8 0.4 0 heterosexual bisexual homosexual asexual

heterosexual

bisexual

homosexual

asexual

c

3.5 2.8

d
Autonomic arousal

5 4 3 2 1 0

Anxiety

2.1 1.4 0.7 0

heterosexual

bisexual

homosexual

asexual

heterosexual

bisexual

homosexual

asexual

123

Arch Sex Behav (2011) 40:699–712

707

erotic film in all groups except the asexuals, where there was no significant increase (Fig. 3d). Physiological-Subjective Concordance Difference scores between erotic and neutral stimulus conditions were calculated for subjective sexual arousal and perceived physical sexual arousal. A percent change score was calculated for VPA ((erotic - neutral)/neutral). The Pearson product-moment correlation was then calculated between these transformed variables and examined in each group separately. Among the asexual women, there was a strong positive correlation between VPA and subjective sexual arousal (p = .036) and perceived genital arousal (p = .05). As can be seen by the scatterplots for the asexual women, these results were not accounted for by outliers, and instead, represent a relatively linear pattern (Fig. 4). Correlations between VPA and selfreport measures were not significant for any of the other three subgroups (Table 2).

Discussion Summary of Findings Overall, the findings revealed no significant differences between asexual women and heterosexual, homosexual, or bisexual women in the genital arousal response to a heterosexual audiovisual erotic stimulus in a controlled, laboratory environment. Instead, all women showed a significant increase in response to the erotic film. A main effect of Film was also found for subjective sexual arousal in that all groups showed a significant increase, with no significant group differences. On perceived genital arousal, there was a trend towards a Group 9 Film interaction such that there was less of an increase among the asexuals compared to the other groups; however, this did not reach statistical significance. Given the small effect size, this effect might not be magnified with a larger sample. As predicted, there was a significant Group 9 Film interaction on the sensuality-sexual attraction domain of the Film Scale such that there was a significant increase following the erotic film in all groups except the asexual women. On other self-report measures of affect, there was a significant Group 9 Film interaction for positive affect to the erotic stimulus such that asexuals did not show the increase in positive affect with the erotic stimulus seen by the other groups. There were no main effects of group or film on negative affect, and anxiety decreased by a similar magnitude for all women with exposure to the erotic stimulus. Interestingly, there was a significant Group 9 Film interaction for autonomic arousal in that all women showed a significant increase with the erotic film except the asexuals.

Fig. 4 Scatterplots of a percent change in VPA vs. subjective sexual arousal, and b percent change in VPA vs. self-reported physical sexual arousal for asexual women (n = 7)

Genital Arousal Responses The lack of a significant Group 9 Film interaction on VPA cannot only be attributed to low sample sizes given that the effect size (partial g2) was small. Nonetheless, these results should be replicated in a larger sample of asexuals. In women, physiological measures such as VPA (Brotto et al., 2004; Pieterse et al., 2008) and fMRI (Arnow et al., 2009) have been found to differentiate women with and without sexual dysfunction in some studies but not in others (Laan, van Driel, & van Lunsen, 2008). Our findings lend support to the speculation (Bogaert, 2006; Brotto et al., 2010; Pagan Westfall, 2004) that asexuality is not a sexual dysfunction—or perhaps, more precisely, it is not a disorder of sexual arousal. In studies exploring VPA differences with regard to sexual orientation, heterosexual and homosexual women did not differ, regardless of the stimulus type shown (Chivers & Bailey, 2005; Chivers

123

708 Table 2 Correlations between percent change in VPA and perception of genital sexual arousal and subjective sexual arousal as a function of group Note: Values represent Pearson product moment correlation coefficients. df in parentheses

Arch Sex Behav (2011) 40:699–712

Perception of genital sexual arousal Heterosexual Bisexual Homosexual Asexual r(9) = .327 r(9) = -.229 r(10) = -.392 r(6) = .753

p

Subjective sexual arousal r(9) = -.038 r(9) = -.108 r(10) = -.247 r(6) = .787

p

ns ns ns .05

ns ns ns .036

et al., 2004, 2007; Peterson, Janssen, & Laan, 2010). That asexual women responded to heterosexual stimuli despite their stated preference for no sexual partners is consistent with the target non-specificity seen in hetero- and homosexual women. Specifically, work by Chivers et al. has consistently shown that, among women, there is the same (strong) magnitude of VPA among different sexual orientation groups to stimuli depicting male–male, female–female, and male–female actor pairings, despite the fact that self-reported arousal levels corresponded with the stimuli of their stated preference. Our data also showed that bisexual women—who previously have not been the subject of VPA research–had the same magnitude of VPA increase as all other groups. If our speculation that target non-specificity is a phenomenon as pertinent to asexual women as it is to women of other sexual orientation groups, then this also suggests that conclusions drawn about the automaticity of women’s sexual responding, which may have evolved to be adaptations to promote facile genital penetration and minimize physical damage (Chivers et al., 2007), also extend to asexual women. Furthermore, one might also speculate that asexual women’s VPA responses to male-male and female-female erotic stimuli would be as robust as their currently observed responses to male-female stimuli, and would not differ from heterosexual and homosexual women. Subjective Arousal Responses There were no significant group differences in sexual arousal when measured by the self-report FSFI or when measured in response to a laboratory stimulus. However, on the DASA, which assesses different components of sexual arousal (i.e., mental sexual arousal vs. genital tingling vs. lubrication vs. pleasant genital sensations), asexual women reported significantly lower levels than the other three groups. These data suggest that with a competent sexual stimulus (such as the conditions during the psychophysiological assessment), asexuals showed a robust subjective sexual arousal response that did not significantly differ from non-asexuals. Moreover, because FSFI arousal ratings were based only on those (asexuals) who were sexually active (57%), the lack of significant group differences suggests that asexuality does not interfere with a sexually active woman’s ability to become aroused. The arousal patterns of sexually inactive women are unknown, and it is possible that if all women were included in the FSFI arousal

domain analysis, lower scores would have been revealed in this group. Since all women were included in analyses of laboratoryinduced subjective arousal and this showed an overall significant increase, this suggests that, regardless of current sexual activity, asexual women are capable of experiencing subjective sexual arousal in response to a sexual stimulus. One recent study that examined subjective sexual arousal based on the degree of‘‘absorption’’(i.e., the ability to immerse oneself in a stimulus without being distracted by other competing stimuli; Koukounas & McCabe, 1997) in the erotic film found that those women who were given instructions to become more absorbed in the film showed higher levels of subjective sexual arousal compared to those who were instructed to view as a passive observer (Sheen & Koukounas, 2009). Because lack of absorption has been associated with difficulties with information processing and possibly attentional disruption (Cranston-Cuebas & Barlow, 1990), our findings suggest that these cognitive and attentional pathways, which are important for sexual responding, are intact in asexual women, and any stated apathy towards sexual stimuli or activity is not due to faulty cognitive or attentive processes. There was a significant Group 9 Film interaction on sensuality-sexual attraction in the predicted direction with all women except the asexuals showing a significant increase to the erotic stimulus. The individual items comprising this subscale include: ‘‘a desire to be close to someone’’and‘‘sexually attractive.’’Given that asexuals define themselves according to a lack of sexual attraction (Jay, 2005), our findings suggest that despite asexuals’ normal sexual arousal response to competent stimuli, they do not experience the corresponding motivation to direct that excitement towards a potential sexual mate. Clarity on this may be gleaned by considering the incentive motivation model of sexual response, which posits that each individual has a sexual response system that can be activated by competent sexual stimuli (Both, Everaerd, & Laan, 2007; Everaerd & Laan, 1995). A component of the model is the disposition to respond to those stimuli with some form of sexual activity, which, in and of itself, then generates sexual desire. This‘‘disposition’’ is likely influenced by central and peripheral neurotransmitter-hormone interactions (Laan & Both, 2008), and can be measured by reflexes of the Achilles tendon, reflecting an ‘‘action tendency’’ (Both, van Boxtel, Stekelenburg, Everaerd, & Laan, 2005). The model views sexual desire as an

123

Arch Sex Behav (2011) 40:699–712

709

emergent property of having engaged in sexual activity—and not the opposite. For asexuals, our data support the speculations of Bogaert (2006), which suggest intact arousability to sexual cues; however, there are likely problems in how this experienced arousability is translated into sexual activity. The resulting lack of desire, therefore, is expected. If the incentive motivation model applies to asexuals, this explains, then, why physiological sexual arousal is intact (reflecting normal arousability to competent sexual stimuli), why sexual desire is reduced (because desire emerges after sexual activity), and suggests that future research should explore the psychological and neurobiological underpinnings of why the activated sexual response does not translate into a sexual action tendency as it does in non-asexuals (Both, Spiering, Everaerd, & Laan, 2004). Taken together, the findings of no group differences on subjective sexual arousal to the erotic film (all women) and no group differences on recalled sexual arousal (for only recently sexually active women) suggest that none of the women in this study experienced impairments in subjective sexual arousal. In other words, asexual women in this study are not likely to meet criteria for a Subjective Sexual Arousal Disorder according to criteria proposed by the American Urological Association Foundation (Basson et al., 2003). Concordance Between Genital and Subjective Sexual Arousal The significant genital-subjective sexual arousal correlation seen in the asexual group but not in the other groups was a novel and intriguing finding. It suggests that with increases in genital sexual arousal, there was a corresponding increase in subjective arousal seen only in asexual women. A meta-analysis exploring sex differences in concordance concluded, after reviewing 134 studies, that correlations were significantly lower for women (r = .26) than they were for men (r = .66) (Chivers et al., 2010). Each of the studies included in the metaanalysis, however, was based on sexual, and not asexual, women. In the present study, the correlation between VPA and subjective arousal was 0.79 and with perceived genital arousal was 0.75—magnitudes much more similar to concordance rates seen in men. Thus, Chivers et al.’s conclusion that‘‘low concordance between self-reported and genital sexual arousal may be the norm for many women’’(p. 46) may apply only to sexual, but not to asexual women. Because higher interoceptive awareness (i.e., awareness of sensations originating from within the body) in men has been cited as a possible reason for sex differences in concordance (Chivers et al., 2010), our data suggest that asexual women may be more interoceptively aware of genital response than are sexual women. At first glance, this may make the findings with regard to lower selfreported autonomic arousal in asexuals seem contradictory. However, it is possible that the three sexual groups misinterpreted their sexual arousal as indicating an autonomic arousal

response whereas the asexual group–being more interoceptively aware—accurately reported a low autonomic response as such. Why might asexual women be more interoceptively aware than sexual women (similar to men)? Perhaps the lack of sexual activity among asexuals leads them to be highly attuned to the (rare) times when they do experience genital arousal. By comparison, heterosexual, homosexual, and bisexual women, who are accustomed to becoming sexually aroused during desired sexual events, may be less attuned to subtle increases in genital arousal evoked in a laboratory. If this line of reasoning is correct, it suggests that the higher level of interoceptive awareness in asexual women had a direct impact on their degree of cognitive sexual excitement, just as is seen in men where self-reported and genital arousal response are highly related. Do Group Differences in Affect Account for the Findings? The significant group difference in positive affect to the erotic film was expected and suggests that, whereas for sexual women, the erotic stimulus was perceived as being associated with pleasure, interest, attraction, excitement, and femininity, the asexual women did not experience any such increase. This finding was consistent with previous findings, which showed that asexuals discuss sexual arousal (and masturbation) using technical, emotionally bare descriptions (Brotto et al., 2010). Asexuals provided non-sexual explanations for why they engaged in masturbation, and the latter was more associated with tension relief than with sexual pleasure. The lack of group differences in negative affect suggests that the arousal generated by the erotic stimuli did not evoke more negative affect for asexual women as one might have expected. Thus, it is not the case that sexual stimuli are perceived negatively (as is the case with Sexual Aversion Disorder; APA, 2000); rather, sexual stimuli were perceived with no increase in positive and no increase in negative affect, i.e., they were perceived neutrally and non-sexually. Is Asexuality a Disorder of Sexual Desire? In this study, sexual desire, as measured by the FSFI, was significantly lower in asexuals compared to the other three groups. Because sexual desire is defined on the FSFI as ‘‘a feeling that includes wanting to have a sexual experience, feeling receptive to a partner’s sexual initiation, and thinking or fantasizing about having sex’’ (Rosen et al., 2000), it is not surprising that asexuals, who lack sexual attraction, responded significantly lower than the other three groups (who presumably do have sexual interest directed towards someone else). Similarly, the Dyadic subscale of the SDI was significantly lower in asexuals. However, asexuals’ scores on the Solitary

123

710

Arch Sex Behav (2011) 40:699–712

SDI subscale did not significantly differ from the sexual groups of women, and were consistent with that of Prause and Graham (2007). Taken together, these findings suggest that desire for sexual activity with another individual is significantly lower in asexual women compared to other sexual orientation groups; however, desire for solitary sexual activity was the same in asexual women compared to sexual women, and was consistent with the masturbation frequency data. Although the significantly lower levels of partner-related sexual desire may suggest that asexuals fit the criteria for HSDD, the fact that they do not experience distress (Criterion B in DSM-IV-TR; APA, 2000) means that they do not meet diagnostic criteria. Furthermore, following from incentive motivation theory (Everaerd & Laan, 1995), which presumes that sexual desire follows sexual activity, the absence of desire in asexuals is expected in light of their absent/infrequent sexual activities. Limitations Our asexual sample was small (n = 7) and this represents a potential limitation of the findings. Another limitation relates to the use of heterosexual stimuli only. Because a more complete test of target non-specificity would necessitate showing a variety of sexual stimuli to participants, this suggests that some of our conclusions regarding the universality of target nonspecificity in women should be understood to be speculations based on the data presented. It is also important to note that our sample of asexual women may not be representative of all asexual women given that they agreed to the viewing of erotic stimuli. Because asexuality is defined as lack of sexual attraction, there may not be an incentive to voluntarily participate in any experimental procedure that involved presentation of undesired stimuli. However, because asexuals are motivated to participate in research that would lead to a greater understanding of asexuality (Jay, 2005), we do not believe that our findings lack external validity. In fact, because we used a variety of different advertisements, some of which mentioned ‘‘asexuality’’and others which instead focused on‘‘do you lack sexual attraction?’’ we believe that we recruited women who may have been at varying levels in their degree of awareness and acceptance of their asexuality, thereby enhancing the external validity of the findings. Conclusions Overall, we found a significant psychophysiological sexual response in the laboratory among asexual women that did not differ from responses seen in groups of heterosexual, bisexual, and homosexual women. Moreover, subjective sexual response did not differ between the groups, but self-reported sensuality-sexual attraction in response to the erotic film was significantly lower among the asexual women, as expected.

Interestingly, concordance between genital and subjective sexual arousal was significantly positive for asexual women, and non-significant for the other groups of women, suggesting greater interoceptive awareness of genital excitement in asexual women. The findings have implications for our conceptualization of asexuality and suggest that, contrary to recent media speculations, asexuality may not be a sexual dysfunction given that arousability pathways appear intact (insofar as our measures test them). Moreover, because the proposed DSM-5 criteria for Sexual Interest/Arousal Disorder require the presence of distress or impairment (Brotto, 2010; Graham, 2010), asexuality would not fall under this definition given asexuals’ lack of distress. The clinical implication of this conclusion means that the asexual woman who is prompted to seek sex therapy at the insistence of a distressed (sexual) partner should not be the recipient of taught sexual skills to boost sexual desire nor should she be a target for pharmacological and/or hormonal intervention to boost desire. Rather, the (sexual/asexual) couple might be the focus of relationship therapy aimed at how to negotiate her lack of sexual attraction and what agreements can be reached about sexual activity that sufficiently appease both partners (e.g., agreeing that sexual activity may take place consensually but without interest for it on her part). Taken together, these results add to the small but growing literature on asexuality. What remains to be studied are whether there are neural correlates to the lack of sexual attraction, whether there are other (non-sexual) biological indicators pointing to asexuality being a sexual orientation (e.g., digit ratio measurement), and further understanding the mechanisms by which romantic and sexual aspects of attraction may become de-coupled. The scientific study of asexuality might also be advanced by systematic comparisons of asexuality (which is lifelong) versus acquired HSDD in order to understand how these populations might be distinct.
Acknowledgments Funding for this study was from University of British Columbia Hampton Research Grant awarded to L. A. Brotto. We wish to thank Yvonne Erskine for assistance in data collection.

References
20/20. (2006). Abstinence forever [video clip]. Available from http:// www.youtube.com/watch?v=CeKGOMUVU7g&feature=PlayList &p=FD6E45790348DC2F&playnext=1&playnext_from=PL& index=30. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author. Arnow, B. A., Millheiser, L., Garrett, A., Lake Polan, M., Glover, G. H., Hill, K. R., et al. (2009). Women with hypoactive sexual desire disorder compared to normal females: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroscience, 158, 484–502. Basson, R., & Brotto, L. A. (2003). Sexual psychophysiology and effects of sildenafil citrate in oestrogenised women with acquired genital

123

Arch Sex Behav (2011) 40:699–712 arousal disorder and impaired orgasm: A randomised controlled trial. British Journal of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, 110, 1014– 1024. Basson, R., Leiblum, S., Brotto, L., Derogatis, L., Fourcroy, J., FuglMeyer, K., et al. (2003). Definitions of women’s sexual dysfunction reconsidered: Advocating expansion and revision. Journal of Psychosomatic Obstetrics and Gynaecology, 24, 221–229. Bogaert, A. F. (2004). Asexuality: Prevalence and associated factors in a national probability sample. Journal of Sex Research, 41, 279–287. Bogaert, A. F. (2006). Toward a conceptual understanding of asexuality. Review of General Psychology, 10, 241–250. Both, S., Everaerd, W., & Laan, E. (2007). Desire emerges from excitement: A psychophysiological perspective on sexual motivation. In E. Janssen (Ed.), The psychophysiology of sex (pp. 327–339). Bloomington: Indiana University Press. Both, S., Spiering, M., Everaerd, W., & Laan, E. (2004). Sexual behavior and responsiveness to sexual stimuli following laboratory-induced sexual arousal. Journal of Sex Research, 41, 242–258. Both, S., van Boxtel, G., Stekelenburg, J., Everaerd, W., & Laan, E. (2005). Modulation of spinal reflexes by sexual films of increasing intensity. Psychophysiology, 42, 726–731. Brotto, L. A. (2009). The Female Sexual Function Index [Letter to the Editor]. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 35, 161–163. Brotto, L. A. (2010). The DSM diagnostic criteria for hypoactive sexual desire disorder in women. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 221–239. Brotto, L. A., Basson, R., & Gorzalka, B. B. (2004). Psychophysiological assessment in premenopausal sexual arousal disorder. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 1, 266–277. Brotto, L. A., Basson, R., & Luria, M. (2008). A mindfulness-based group psychoeducational intervention targeting sexual arousal disorder in women. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 5, 1646–1659. Brotto, L. A., Knudson, G., Inskip, J., Rhodes, K., & Erskine, Y. (2010). Asexuality: A mixed-methods approach. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 599–618. Chivers, M. L., & Bailey, J. M. (2005). A sex difference in features that elicit genital response. Biological Psychology, 70, 115–120. Chivers, M. L., Rieger, G., Latty, E., & Bailey, J. M. (2004). A sex difference in the specificity of sexual arousal. Psychological Science, 15, 736–744. Chivers, M. L., Seto, M. C., & Blanchard, R. (2007). Gender and sexual orientation differences in sexual response to sexual activities versus gender of actors in sexual films. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 1108–1121. Chivers, M., Seto, M., Lalumiere, M., Laan, E., & Grimbos, T. (2010). Agreement of self-reported and genital measures of sexual arousal in men and women: A meta-analysis. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 5–56. CNN Showbiz Tonight. (2006). A look at asexuality [video clip]. Available from http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=kkoo1lUp3ns. Cranston-Cuebas, M. A., & Barlow, D. H. (1990). Cognitive and affective contributions to sexual functioning. Annual Review of Sex Research, 1, 119–161. Everaerd, W., & Laan, E. (1995). Desire for passion: Energetics of sexual response. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 21, 255–263. Fox News Dayside. (2006). Sex-free romance [video clip]. Available from http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=lmXXgRvotGM. Graham, C. A. (2010). The DSM diagnostic criteria for female sexual arousal disorder. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 240–255. Heiman, J. R. (1977). A psychophysiological exploration of sexual arousal patterns in females and males. Psychophysiology, 14, 266–274. Heiman, J. R., & Rowland, D. L. (1983). Affective and physiological sexual response patterns: The effects of instructions on sexually functional and dysfunctional men. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 27, 105–116.

711 Herbenick, D., Reece, M., Sanders, S., Dodge, B., Ghassemi, A., & Fortenberry, J. D. (2009). Prevalence and characteristics of vibrator use by women in the United States: Results from a nationally representative study. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 6, 1856– 1866. Hoon, P. W., Wincze, J. P., & Hoon, E. F. (1976). Physiological assessment of sexual arousal in women. Psychophysiology, 13, 196–204. Janssen, E., Vorst, H., Finn, P., & Bancroft, J. (2002). The Sexual Inhibition (SIS) and Sexual Excitation (SES) Scales: I. Measuring sexual inhibition and excitation proneness in men. Journal of Sex Research, 39, 114–126. Jay, D. (2003). A look at online collective identity formation. Unpublished manuscript. Jay, D. (2005). Asexuality visibility and education network. Retrieved February 20, 2006, from http://asexuality.org/home/content/section/ 6/28/index.php?option=com_content&task=section&id=6&Itemid =28. Koukounas, E., & McCabe, M. (1997). Sexual and emotional variables influencing sexual response to erotica. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 221–230. Laan, E., & Both, S. (2008). What makes women experience desire? Feminism and Psychology, 18, 505–514. Laan, E., Everaerd, W., & Evers, A. (1995). Assessment of female sexual arousal: Response specificity and construct validity. Psychophysiology, 32, 476–485. Laan, E., van Driel, E. M., & van Lunsen, R. H. W. (2008). Genital responsiveness in healthy women with and without Sexual Arousal Disorder. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 5, 1424–1435. Masters, W. H., & Johnson, V. E. (1966). Human sexual response. Boston: Little Brown. Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F., & Dolezal, C. (2007). The Female Sexual Function Index: A methodological critique and suggestions for improvement. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 33, 217–224. Montel Williams Show. (2007). Asexuality [video clip]. Available from http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=ALPwob9b9qE&feature= related. Pagan Westfall, S. (2004). Glad to be A. New Scientist, 184, 40–43. Peterson, Z. D., Janssen, E., & Laan, E. (2010). Women’s sexual responses to heterosexual and lesbian erotica: The role of stimulus intensity, affective reaction, and sexual history. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 807–880. Pieterse, Q. D., Ter Kuile, M. M., Deruiter, M. C., Trimbos, J. B. M. Z., Kenter, G. G., & Maas, C. P. (2008). Vaginal blood flow after radical hysterectomy with and without nerve sparing. A preliminary report. International Journal of Gynecologic Cancer, 18, 576–583. Prause, N., & Graham, C. A. (2007). Asexuality: Classification and characterization. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36, 341–356. Pretzelboy. (2009, August). Asexual explorations blog [Web log message]. Retrieved from http://asexystuff.blogspot.com/2009/ 05/is-asexuality-sexual-dysfunction.html). Rieger, G., Chivers, M. L., & Bailey, J. M. (2005). Sexual arousal patterns of bisexual men. Psychological Science, 16, 579–584. Rosen, R., Brown, C., Heiman, J., Leiblum, S., Meston, C., Shabsigh, R., et al. (2000). The Female Sexual Function Index (FSFI): A multidimensional self-report instrument for the assessment of female sexual function. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 26, 191–208. Rosen, R. C., Riley, A., Wagner, G., Osterloh, I. H., Kirkpatrick, J., & Mishra, A. (1997). The International Index of Erectile Function (IIEF): A multidimensional scale for assessment of erectile dysfunction. Urology, 49, 822–830. Rothblum, E. D., & Brehony, K. A. (1993). Boston marriages: Romantic but asexual relationships among contemporary lesbians. Amherst: University of Massachusetts Press.

123

712 Scherrer, K. S. (2008). Coming to an asexual identity: Negotiating identity, negotiating desire. Sexualities, 11, 621–641. Sears, J. T. (2006). Behind the mask of the Mattachine: The Hal Call chronicles and the early movement for homosexual emancipation. New York: Harrington Park Press. Sheen, J., & Koukounas, E. (2009). The role of absorption in women’s sexual response to erotic: A cognitive-affective investigation. Journal of Sex Research, 46, 358–365. Sintchak, G., & Geer, J. H. (1975). A vaginal plethysmograph system. Psychophysiology, 12, 113–115. Spector, I. P., Carey, M. P., & Steinberg, L. (1996). The Sexual Desire Inventory: Development, factor structure and evidence of reliability. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 22, 175–190. Storms, M. D. (1980). Theories of sexual orientation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 783–792.

Arch Sex Behav (2011) 40:699–712 The View. (2006). Secrets of asexuals [video clip]. Available from http:// www.youtube.com/watch?v=6kPfLYuQlL8. Tiefer, L. (2002). Sexual behaviour and its medicalisation. Many (especially economic) forces promote medicalisation. British Medical Journal, 325, 45. Tiefer, L. (2006). Sex therapy as a humanistic enterprise. Sexual and Relationship Therapy, 21, 359–375. Tucker Carlson. (2006). Under the radar-asexuality [video clip]. Available from http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=Iwxo6t7XBYs. Wiegel, M., Meston, C., & Rosen, R. (2005). The Female Sexual Function Index (FSFI): Cross-validation and development of clinical cutoff scores. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 31, 1–20. Wincze, J. P., Albert, A., & Bansal, S. (1993). Sexual arousal in diabetic females: Physiological and self-report measures. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 22, 587–601.

123

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Similar Documents

Premium Essay

Business to Business

...Social Networks and Media Conquer B2B Social Networks are a flourishing phenomenon in today’s generation. It has already entered and affected the B2C network. In B2C network the social networks have become a concerning factor and plays a major role in marketing and communications. As we are all familiar with the B2B network and what does it actually implies, the question arises whether the social networks can play an important role in B2B network and for B2B companies. Social networks are not another passing phenomenon; they have matured and have registered themselves highly in the minds of the customers. B2B network also gets conquered by the social networks the business houses that trade with each other are actually the customers of each other and they also engage themselves in social networking. Some useful statistics from the social networking world states that the total time spent on social media has increased from 6% in 2007 to 20% in 2011. The registered users will almost reach 2 billion people by 2014. These figures show relentless growth and usage of social media. Already 91% of the B2B companies are active on Facebook, 53% on Twitter and 47% on LinkedIn. These statistics reveal that social media has conquered B2B network and has entered in that chain as means of effective communication and presentation. B2B companies deal with people and not robots. So the influence of emotions before purchasing a product is applicable to them also as they are not immune to these...

Words: 798 - Pages: 4

Premium Essay

Business

...Unit Title: The Business Environment Guided Learning Hours: 160 Level: Level 5 Number of Credits: 18 Learning Outcome 1 The learner will: Understand the significance of the environment on business activity. Assessment Criteria The learner can: 1.1 Explain the features of business activity. Indicative Content 1.1.1 Explain the business organisation as a transformation process. 1.1.2 Describe the different ways in which a business may structure its activities. 1.2 Discuss the complex interactions that operate between a business and its environment. 1.2.1 Discuss the complexity, volatility and uniqueness of an organisation's environment. 1.2.2 Explain the influence of shareholders on the internal environment of an organisation. 1.2.3 Outline the nature of the external environment (PEST). 1.3 Explain the need for change and its management within an individual business. 1.3.1 Discuss the need to monitor changes in the external environment. 1.3.2 Discuss the implications of environmental change for a business, its stakeholders and society. 1.3.3 Explain reasons for resistance to change by firms, managers and employees. 1.3.4 Describe ways in which resistance to change may be overcome. Learning Outcome 2 The learner will: Understand the structure and organisation of business. Assessment Criteria The learner can: 2.1 Discuss the impact of industrial and legal structure and size on organisational behaviour. Indicative Content 2.1.1 Classify businesses...

Words: 1138 - Pages: 5

Premium Essay

Business

...The Business Enterprise 1.) Discuss the common elements described in the theories/philosophies of Case, Kouzes, and Drucker including how their principles/strategies relate to the new definition of entrepreneurial leadership presented in Understanding Entrepreneurial Leadership in today’s Dynamic Markets. Case, Kouzes and Drucker each have good theories and philosophies on entrepreneurship and leadership. Each had different ways of stating what they thought made a good entrepreneur but for the most part focus on some of the same things and agreed that with the right balance of these characteristics then succeeding, as an entrepreneur would be possible. One of the common elements that each focused on was people. Interaction with people is important when it comes to entrepreneurship and leadership skills according to Case, Kouzes, and Drucker. Each inferred that if a person does not have people skills, then the person would not succeed as an entrepreneur or a leader. People skills would be the ability to motivate others and interact. A leader cannot lead without being able to interact with people. Starting a business requires interaction with people on some level. Another common element that they all thought were important was not being afraid to take risk. Taking a risk in order to monopolize an industry to succeed is important. The definition of entrepreneurial leadership is one of an enterprising, transformational leader who operates in a dynamic...

Words: 1383 - Pages: 6

Premium Essay

Business

...Business communication can take many forms, written and verbal. It can be between managers and employees within a company, or between a company and its customers, partners or suppliers. The purpose of business communication also varies, but it should always benefit your business. When carried out effectively, business communication can build your company's reputation, resolve and prevent conflicts, and contribute to strong relationships between your company, its customers and the business community. Build Customer Relationships Few businesses would grow or even survive without the benefits gained through marketing communications. You can take advantage of the wide variety of media available to let the public know about your products or services. Use business communications to establish your company as an authority or thought leader in your industry. Offer a white paper or online articles that discuss common problems business owners face and present your company's solution. A weekly or monthly newsletter to your prospects or established customers will help build trust by providing helpful tips and information. Occasionally sending out a direct mail or email survey will help you get to know your customers and better fill their needs. Freely sharing your expertise and gathering feedback from your customers will help you build a strong relationship with your customer base, and establish your company as a trusted expert in your industry. Motivate Team Good communication...

Words: 763 - Pages: 4

Premium Essay

Business

...ecutive Summary The New Australian Company is a new red wine brand that will be introduced in Singapore. The brand is a unique and high quality red wine brand that meets the growing preference of consumers. It will be distributed to several hotels, restaurants, supermarkets, and wine boutiques/shops that are located in the commercial business districts of the city-state. The brand is managed and owned by a group of wine-lover businessmen who have tremendous experienced in business administration, marketing and sales management and customer service. The owners are now looking for additional investments in order to capitalize the project well. The company has already made several negotiations with successful business distributors in Singapore. The company is confident to compete with many large distributors because it is closely working with top-notch suppliers and manufacturers who are known as wine experts or cellar masters. The target market is the Generation Y or the millennial because it is 77 percent of the population. This group also consumes wines at a faster and higher rate. The sales forecasts for the New Australian Company begin with $700,000 for the first year, and this will increase to 50 percent ($1, 050,000) in the second year, and 60 percent ($1,680,000) in the third year of operation. The company will be expecting a profit margin of 30-40 percent, and at least 25 percent return of investment in the first operational year. The company wants to change...

Words: 1216 - Pages: 5

Premium Essay

Business

...Module-16 The role of business in The economy: M arkets and coMMerce TEACHER’S GUIDE P. 487 P. 491 P. 492 P. 492 P. 493 P. 496 P. 497 P. 501 Defined Content standards Materials Procedure Lesson outline Closure Assessment Overheads VISUAlS N Visuals for overhead projector. Copy to transparent paper for overhead. P. 502 NVisual-1: Market defined P. 503 NVisual-2: Business defined lESSonS 2 Copy and handout to students. P. 506 P. 513 P. 518 P. 522 2Lesson-I: I, Pencil 2Lesson-II: Specializing in production 2Lesson-III: Wal-Mart 2Lesson assessment the role of business in the econoMy Markets and coMMerce Module-16 Teacher DEFInED A market is a social arrangement that allows buyers and sellers to discover information and complete voluntary exchanges of goods and services. commerce consists of trading something of economic value such as a good, service, information, or money between two entities. business is the management and coordination of people and resources to accomplish particular production goals, usually for the purpose of making profit. business, commerce, and markets create many benefits for consumers, and as a result of business, commerce, and markets in the economy, consumers experience an increased standard of living. businesses obtain resources such as labor and equipment, and businesses specialize in the production of particular goods and services. As a result of business in the economy, the variety...

Words: 9175 - Pages: 37

Premium Essay

Business

...cover cover next page > Cover Business, Management and Finance Small Business; Entrepreneurship title author publisher isbn10 | asin print isbn13 ebook isbn13 language subject publication date lcc ddc subject : : : : : : : Entrepreneurship and Small Business Burns, Paul. 0333914732 9780333914731 9781403917102 : : : : cover next page > file:///Z|/_==%CF%CE%C8%D1%CA==/Entrepreneurship%.../0333914732__gigle.ws/0333914732/files/cover.html [06.10.2009 1:01:58] page_i < previous page page_i next page > page_i next page > Page i Entrepreneurship and Small Business < previous page file:///Z|/_==%CF%CE%C8%D1%CA==/Entrepreneurship%...0333914732__gigle.ws/0333914732/files/page_i.html [06.10.2009 1:02:00] page_ii < previous page page_ii next page > page_ii next page > Page ii < previous page file:///Z|/_==%CF%CE%C8%D1%CA==/Entrepreneurship%2...0333914732__gigle.ws/0333914732/files/page_ii.html [06.10.2009 1:02:00] page_iii < previous page page_iii next page > page_iii next page > Page iii Entrepreneurship and Small Business Paul Burns < previous page file:///Z|/_==%CF%CE%C8%D1%CA==/Entrepreneurship%2...333914732__gigle.ws/0333914732/files/page_iii.html [06.10.2009 1:02:00] page_iv < previous page page_iv next page > Page iv © Paul Burns 2001 All rights reserved. No reproduction, copy or transmission of this publication may be...

Words: 182024 - Pages: 729

Premium Essay

Business

...CHANGING CLOTHES. CHANGING CONVENTIONAL WISDOM. CHANGING THE WORLD. Fast Retailing has the conviction and vision to face the challenge of creating a better world. PARIS Valerie Dassier, Head of E-Commerce and Customer Care, Comptoire des Cottonniers and Princess Tam Tam To become a leader, it’s not enough to do it a little bit better. Great leaders see a different reality. LONDON Kate Pierre, Store Manager, Uniqlo UK We are going to be the number one casual clothes company in the world. And we will look to our managers in the company to take on bigger roles in their local countries and abroad. MOSCOW Aleksandr Kurchatov, Store Manager, Uniqlo Your attitude and your smile go a long way. My philosophy is to give people the chance to smile and they will open up. NEW YORK Jennifer Parker, VP Store Management, Theory, NY More than any other retailer, FR empowers people to be the CEOs of their own four walls. They put the onus on the store manager to change themselves and make it the most important role. SAN FRANCISCO Daisuke Tsukakoshi, Director West Coast, Uniqlo USA In the U.S., the home of casual clothing, competition is fierce. We’ll need people who are willing to play major roles in opening up this effort. SHANGHAI Joanne Lam, Merchandising Director, Uniqlo, Hong Kong and Mainland China I always admired Coco Channel as a kid. She turned around the whole fashion industry of her time. I want to work in a big company that is prepared to make that...

Words: 6333 - Pages: 26

Premium Essay

Business

...- BTEC FIRST DIPLOMA BUSINESS - Unit 16: Business Enterprise Assignment [pic] [pic] • The due date for the whole assignment is Friday 25th May 2012 • You will have time allocated to complete the MERIT and DISTINCTION criteria only when the PASS criteria is completed • You will be able to gather evidence for Level 2 Key Skills in Communication, Application of Number and Information Technology from completing the assignment You will also be able to gather evidence for Key Skills Level 2 Improving Own Learning and Performance (LP2.1, Lp2.2 and LP2.3) as you plan, review and complete the assignment ASSIGNMENT TASKS The scenario You and some friends have always been interested in working for yourself rather than being an employee. You now feel it is the right time to start up a new business and so you begin to consider the idea and investigate the possibilities further with your friends. There are many ideas you have but before you can start the business you have decided to investigate what things you need to know about starting up a business. You and your friends are considering asking your bank manager to provide you with the finance for starting up the business and so it is important that you know exactly what is involved. You and your friends will have to present your business idea to the bank manager so it is important that you know what you have to do…! ______________________________________________________________ ...

Words: 905 - Pages: 4

Free Essay

Business

...to this issue. The latest issue can be found on the Edexcel website: www.edexcel.com References to third-party material made in this specification are made in good faith. Edexcel does not endorse, approve or accept responsibility for the content of materials, which may be subject to change, or any opinions expressed therein. (Material may include textbooks, journals, magazines and other publications and websites.) Authorised by Roger Beard Prepared by Susan Hoxley Publications Code BH029073 All the material in this publication is copyright © Edexcel Limited 2011 Edexcel BTEC Level 4 HNC Diploma in Business Edexcel BTEC Level 5 HND Diploma in Business Edexcel BTEC Level 5 HND Diploma in Business (Accounting) Edexcel BTEC Level 5 HND Diploma in Business (Management) Edexcel BTEC Level 5 HND Diploma in Business (Marketing) Edexcel BTEC Level 5 HND Diploma in Business (Human...

Words: 15079 - Pages: 61

Premium Essay

Business

...A business undertaking is an organisation which is engaged in some industrial or commercial activity. It represents an institutional arrangement for carrying on any kind of business activity. It may be owned and controlled by a single individual or by a group of individuals who have entered into a formal or informal agreement to jointly conduct the business. Every business undertaking is a separate and distinct business unit. It has its own identity and separate ownership. It can be distinguished from other undertakings on the basis of its ownership, management and control. According to Wheeler, a business undertaking is a concern, company or enterprise which buys and sells, is owned by one person or a group of persons and is managed under a specific set of operating policies". Thus, a business undertaking may be defined as an organisation operating under separate ownership, management and control and carrying on any business activity with independent risk- bearing. All business undertakings are directly or indirectly engaged in the transfer or exchange of goods and services for value. They deal in goods and services on a regular basis. Their main motive is to earn profits and they are exposed to various types of risks. Characteristics of a Business Undertaking The basic features of a business undertaking are as follows: 1. Separate identity: Every business undertaking has a separate identity. It has a distinct name and separate existence. Its assets and liabilities are...

Words: 1147 - Pages: 5

Premium Essay

Business

...Gerri Bednarcyk Ch 1 Assign - Business Now: Change is the Only Constant Respond to these questions in a Word document. You do not have to include the question, but make sure to number the response.  You need to use complete sentences and correct grammar.  Once you have answered all the questions, save it as Chapter 1 – [your name] and upload it as an attachment. Watch the lecture video:  Make sure you have read Chapter 1 and have posted in the discussion forum. 1. What factors contribute to the rapid pace of change in business? Is the pace likely to accelerate or decrease over the next decade? Why? The factors that contribute to the rapid pace of change in business are Human Resources, Capital, Natural Resources, Entrepreneurship, and Technology. The factors are more than likely to increase because all factors listed are increasing at a fast pace. I feel that factors are increasing because they are definitely in high demand at this time in our economy. 2. Explain the term "business".  A business is any organization or activity that provides goods and services in an effort to earn a profit. Businesses are leading in capitalist economies, where most of them are privately owned and ran to earn profit to increase the wealth of their owners. Businesses may also be not-for-profit or state-owned 3. Explain the role of business in the economy. The economies of the country depends upon the employment provided by big and small businesses and produces the goods and provide...

Words: 908 - Pages: 4

Premium Essay

Business

...Recording Transactions Keeping business records accurate and up to date is important for the smooth running of a business. The business owner must record all of the money coming into the business from successful sales of the product and all of the money going out, such as expenses including money owed for storage, wages that are paid and money to purchase the stock. If a business fails to do this it may find itself not chasing payments, forgetting to pay bills or, even in trouble with HM Revenue and Customs. If the business does not record its transactions correctly, it cannot report its financial performance accurately and therefore tax payments may be wrong. By recording the sales made, the business owner is given a clear indication of what’s popular and so it becomes clear what products should have increased stock or altered price. In your case, it is very important that you monitor your sales as a new business will most likely not make any profit straight away, so it’s vital to record your transactions so you are able to at least breakeven until more customers become aware of your business and you can start to make a profit. Furthermore, since you have negotiated a one month credit with a supplier, it is important to record your transactions so you know that you can repay the supplier after the month has passed and avoid any consequences. It would be beneficial to you if you could keep your credit supply as then you will have money coming in from your customers to then...

Words: 274 - Pages: 2

Free Essay

Business

...The United States exists in a capitalistic economy meaning that private enterprise is based on fair competition, economic freedom, and private ownership therefore; business is the heartbeat of our economy. Everything from manufacturing, health care, sales, and agriculture involves business in some way. Businesses provide jobs and produce goods and services that we depend on every day (What Role Does Business Play in Our Economy..?). They pay state and federal taxes to the government on their services provided. The taxes paid by businesses and the people employed by them allow the government to function. Looking at businesses in this way you can clearly see that business is a pivotal part of the economy. Within our economy two types of business organizations exist: for-profit and nonprofit. A for-profit organization will produce goods or services in the hopes of making revenue off its products. Most organizations you think of today are probably a for-profit establishment. Organizations that are classified as not-for-profit or nonprofit often operate under the guise of the emotional appeal of compassion but can also become major economic engines. They are business like establishments but unlike a for-profit business, their primary goals do not include making monetary profits. For profit businesses operate to generate revenue under the idea to have more profit than loss at the end of the day. Unlike for- profit businesses, nonprofits are organizations that are classified...

Words: 1532 - Pages: 7

Premium Essay

Business

...offer the bases in preparing, presenting and displaying even in interpreting general-purpose financial statements. There are some important accounting concepts that support the readiness and preparation of any accounting arrangement or financial statements such as Going Concern Concept, Consistency Concept, Prudence Concept, and Accruals Concepts. For example, Going Concern Concept is a company or organizations will not be going to bankrupt unless there have a confirmation and evidence to the contrary and this is supported by accountants. Purpose of Accounting Conventions An accounting convention refer to regular and common practices which are all around followed in recording and exhibiting accounting data and information of the business entity. They are taken after like traditions, convention, and so forth in a general public. Accounting conventions are evolved through the normal and consistent practice throughout the years to encourage uniform recording in the books of accounts. Accounting conventions help in contrasting accounting information of various specialty units or of the same unit for various...

Words: 645 - Pages: 3