Free Essay

Crickets and Dopamine

In:

Submitted By nkapoor
Words 4412
Pages 18
Cocaine reward and locomotion stimulation in mice with reduced dopamine transporter expression
-------------------------------------------------

-------------------------------------------------

-------------------------------------------------
Abstract
Background
The dopamine transporter (DAT) plays a critical role in regulating dopamine neurotransmission. Variations in DAT or changes in basal dopaminergic tone have been shown to alter behavior and drug responses. DAT is one of the three known high affinity targets for cocaine, a powerful psychostimulant that produces reward and stimulates locomotor activity in humans and animals. We have shown that cocaine no longer produces reward in knock-in mice with a cocaine insensitive mutant DAT (DAT-CI), suggesting that cocaine inhibition of DAT is critical for its rewarding effect. However, in DAT-CI mice, the mutant DAT has significantly reduced uptake activity resulting in elevated basal dopaminergic tone, which might cause adaptive changes that alter responses to cocaine. Therefore, the objective of this study is to determine how elevated dopaminergic tone affects how mice respond to cocaine.
Results
We examined the cocaine induced behavior of DAT knockdown mice that have DAT expression reduced by 90% when compared to the wild type mice. Despite a dramatic reduction of DAT expression and marked elevation in basal dopamine tone, cocaine produced reward, as measured by conditioned place preference, and stimulated locomotor activity in these mice.
Conclusion
A reduction in DAT expression and elevation of dopaminergic tone do not lead to adaptive changes that abolish the rewarding and stimulating effects of cocaine. Therefore, the lack of reward to cocaine observed in DAT-CI mice is unlikely to have resulted from the reduced DAT activity but instead is likely due to the inability of cocaine to block the mutated DAT and increase extracellular dopamine. This study supports the conclusion that the blockade of DAT is required for cocaine reward and locomotor stimulation.
-------------------------------------------------
Background
Cocaine is a powerful psychostimulant and an addictive drug of abuse. Considerable evidence indicates that the blockade of the dopamine transporter (DAT) by cocaine and the subsequent elevation of extracellular dopamine (DA) primarily MEDIATE the stimulating and rewarding effects of cocaine [1-4]. DA is an important neurotransmitter involved in many critical functions includingMOTOR CONTROL, emotion, motivation, memory, and reward [5]. The DA transporter is responsible for DA reuptake and RECYCLING, and thus plays a critical role in maintaining DA homeostasis and in regulating DA neurotransmission. Variations or polymorphisms in the DAT gene have been associated with behavioral changes and modified drug response. For instance, it has been shown that the 9-repeat allele of a 3' variable number tandem repeat polymorphism in the DAT gene is associated with lower basal DA tone and greater DA release in response to nicotine [6]. More significantly, the 9-repeat allele has also been associated with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and increased aggression [7,8].
There is also evidence that changes to DAT in animals can affect their drug responses and behavior. It is widely believed that the addictive psychostimulants elevate locomotor activity and produce reward by activating the dopaminergic system. However, cocaine still produces reward in dopamine transporter knockout mice (DAT-KO mice) [9-11]. Importantly, DAT-KO mice show additional differences from wild type mice. The selective serotonin transporter (SERT) inhibitor fluoxetine and the selective norepinephrine transporter (NET) inhibitor nisoxetine do not produce reward in wild type mice, but both drugs produce reward in DAT-KO mice [12-14]. Fluoxetine and reboxetine (another NET selective inhibitor) have also been reported to increase extracellular DA levels in the nucleus accumbens in DAT-KO mice, but not in wild type mice [13,15,16]. It is likely that the absence of DAT in DAT-KO mice has altered neuronal signaling pathways. Therefore, it is not clear how the modulation of DAT would affect cocaine induced reward and locomotor activity.
To avoid the adaptive changes seen in DAT-KO mice, we previously generated a knock-in mouse line with a cocaine-insensitive DAT (DAT-CI) [17]. In DAT-CI mice, cocaine decreases locomotor activity and does not produce reward as measured by conditioned place preference (CPP) [17]. Our results suggest that the blockade of DAT is required for cocaine induced reward and locomotor stimulation. In DAT-CI mice however, the modified DAT has reduced transport function resulting in elevated extracellular DA levels and hyperactivity. Therefore, it is a concern that cocaine's inability to produce reward or to increase locomotor activity might be due to increased DA basal tone or other adaptations caused by the reduced DA transport function in DAT-CI mice.
A DAT knock-down mouse line (DAT-KD) has been generated which has a 90% reduction in DAT expression compared to the wild type mice [18]. DAT-KD mice display significantly altered behaviors, such as hyperactivity and slower habituation to a novel environment. Importantly, amphetamine, which increases locomotion in wild type mice, reduces locomotor activity in DAT-KD mice [18]. DAT-KD mice also exhibit differences from wild type mice in their response to sweet rewards [19,20]. These studies indicate that alterations in DAT expression can change an animal's behavior and modify its response to drugs including psychostimulants. DAT-KD mice provide a good animal MODEL to test how reduced DAT expression affects cocaine-induced animal behaviors.
In the current study, we compared the cocaine response in DAT-KD mice and wild type mice with two behavioral tests: drug induced locomotion and CPP. Our results revealed the effects of reduced DAT expression on cocaine-elicited animal behaviors.
-------------------------------------------------
Results
Locomotion stimulation
First, we examined the effects of cocaine or saline injection on mouse locomotor activity. WT and DAT-KD mice were placed in an open field box (25 cm × 25 cm) and locomotor activity was recorded for 60 minutes. After the habituation period, cocaine or saline were injected and mouse locomotor activities were monitored for another 60 minutes. Figure 1A and 1B show the time COURSE of the response of DAT-KD and wild type mice, respectively, to saline, 5 mg/kg, 10 mg/kg and 20 mg/kg cocaine. DAT-KD mice displayed higher basal locomotor activity (p < 0.001, t-test) than their wild type littermates (Fig. 1C). This observation is consistent with previously published results [18]. Fig.1D shows the total distance TRAVELED IN 30 min after injection of saline or different doses of cocaine. Two way ANOVA detected a main effect of drug (F3,66 = 22.38, p < 0.001) and significant difference between genotypes (F1,66 = 19.57, p < 0.001); but the drug-genotype interaction did not reach a significant level (F3,66 = 2.686, p = 0.053). The data were further analyzed with post hoc Bonferroni tests. Comparing the two genotypes of mice, we found no difference between WT and DAT-KD mice in distance traveled after saline injection (p > 0.05), despite the higher basal locomotor activity in DAT-KD mice. However, 5 mg/kg and 10 mg/kg cocaine had a greater stimulating effect on locomotor activity in the DAT-KD mice (p < 0.01 and p < 0.001), while there was no significant difference in the effects of 20 mg/kg cocaine on the two genotypes of mice (p > 0.05). For the cocaine effect, post hoc tests indicated that 5, 10, and 20 mg/kg cocaine significantly stimulated locomotor activity compared to saline in the DAT-KD mice (p < 0.01, p < 0.001, and p < 0.001). In the wild type mice, 5 mg/kg cocaine did not significantly stimulate locomotor activity, while 10 and 20 mg/kg cocaine did (p > 0.05, p < 0.001, and p < 0.001 respectively).
Figure 1. Effect of cocaine on locomotor activity in DAT-KD mutant mice compared to WT mice. Mice were habituated to the locomotor test chamber for 60 minutes. Cocaine or saline were injected (ip) and mice were returned to the test chamber and monitored for another 60 min. A) and B): Time COURSE of locomotor activity of WT mice and DAT-KD mice. Saline, 5 mg/kg, 10 mg/kg or 20 mg/kg cocaine was given at the time indicated by the arrows. Data shown are average distance traveled in 5 min. C) Total distance traveled in 60 minutes for wild type and DAT-KD mice during their habituation to the chambers before drug or saline injection. DAT-KD mice are significantly more active than wild type ice (***, p < 0.001, t-test). D) Total distance traveled in 30 min after the injection of saline or 5, 10, or 20 mg/kg cocaine with 6 – 8 mice in each group. Two-way ANOVA was performed. Cocaine significantly increased locomotor activities in both genotypes of mice (for drug effect, F3,66 = 22.38, p < 0.001) and had greater effect on DAT-KD mice than on the WT mice (for genotype, F1,66 = 19.57, p < 0.001). Error bars represent standard error of means. Post hoc Bonferroni tests versus saline: *, p < 0.05; **, p < 0.01; ***, p < 0.001. Comparing between the two genotypes, 5 mg/kg and 10 mg/kg cocaine had a greater effect on locomotor activity in the DAT-KD mice than in wild type mice (#, p < 0.01).
Conditioned place preference
Next, we tested whether cocaine can still produce reward in DAT-KD mice using the CPP test. As shown in Fig. 2, 5 mg/kg and 20 mg/kg cocaine significantly increased the time spent in cocaine-paired chamber by both wild type and DAT-KD mice. Statistical analysis with ANOVA indicated a main drug effect (F2,40 = 24.25, p < 0.0001), the lack of a difference between genotypes (F1,40 = 0.1180, p > 0.05) and the lack of a drug-genotype interaction (F2,40 = 0.8827, p > 0.05). Post-hoc Bonferroni analysis showed the increase in time spent in the cocaine-paired chamber was significant at 5 mg/kg and 20 mg/kg cocaine for both wild type (p < 0.05 and 0.01 respectively) and DAT-KD mice (p < 0.001 and 0.001 respectively). The time mice spent in the unpaired chamber decreased in the cocaine treated groups (data not shown). Therefore, the rewarding effect of cocaine is preserved in DAT-KD mice in which the activity of DAT, the primary target of cocaine, is dramatically reduced.
Figure 2. Cocaine induced CPP in DAT-KD mutant mice and WT mice. CPP is represented by the difference in the time mice spend in the drug-paired chamber between pre-conditioning day and post-conditioning day. Both 5 and 20 mg/kg cocaine produced significant CPP in both WT mice and DAT-KD mice (for drug effect, F2,1 = 24.25, p < 0.0001). Eight mice were examined in each group. Error bars represent standard error of means. Post hoc Bonferroni tests versus saline: *, p < 0.05; **, p < 0.01; ***, p < 0.001.
-------------------------------------------------
Discussion
The dopamine transporter is a key component in the regulation of DA neurotransmission. Most evidence suggests that the blockade of DAT and the subsequent increase in extracellular DA primarilyMEDIATE the stimulating and rewarding effects of cocaine. However, the persistence of the rewarding effect of cocaine in DAT-KO mice suggests a mechanism of DAT-independent cocaine reward, which might be due to the extensive adaptive changes in DAT-KO mice. To test whether the DAT-independent cocaine reward also plays a role in normal mice, we made a knock-in mouse line with the endogenous DAT replaced by a cocaine-resistant DAT mutant. In these DAT-CI mice, cocaine suppresses locomotion and does not produce reward, supporting the hypothesis that cocaine blockade of DAT is required for cocaine reward in normal mice [17]. However, DAT activity in DAT-CI mice is significantly lower than that in wild type mice, and changes in DAT have been shown to alter behaviors or drug responses. For example, reduced DAT activity in DAT-KD mice has been shown to alter behavioral responses to amphetamine, another psychostimulant that impacts the dopaminergic system [18]. It remains a concern that in DAT-CI mice the lack of cocaine response might be due to compensatory changes from the lowered DAT activity. Therefore, we studied cocaine effects on DAT-KD mice.
The DAT-KO and DAT-CI mice were generated using ES cells from 129Sv/J mice and crossed with C57BL6 mice, thus they have a mixed genetic background [17]. Several studies used mice that were backcrossed with C57BL6 mice for many generations [17]. The original DAT-KD mice were generated in 129Sv/J background but this strain does not respond well in many tests that are used to evaluate psychostimulant effects. Therefore, the DAT-KD mice used in this study were backcrossed to C57BL/6J for 8 to 9 generations and thus their background matches with the backgrounds of both the DAT-CI mice and DAT-KO mice. This is important because it has been shown that cocaine's effects are not the same in different strains of mice [21,22]. In addition, DAT-KO mice backcrossed to different strain backgrounds also respond to cocaine differently [23].
In DAT-KD mice, DAT activity is reduced to 10% of the wild type level, resulting in a hyperdopaminergic tone and increased locomotor activity [18]. In addition, DAT-KD mice have an altered response to amphetamine, which might be due to an altered balance between DA autoreceptor and heteroreceptor functions [18]. Amphetamine elevates extracellular DA by interacting with DAT and, more importantly, with the vesicular monoamine transporter, inhibiting DA uptake, and inducing DA release from DA containing vesicles to the cytosol through the vesicular monoamine transporter and to the extracellular space through DAT [5]. In contrast, cocaine elevates extracellular DA by simply binding to DAT and inhibiting DA reuptake. Despite the vastly different mechanisms of action, amphetamine and cocaine both increase DA in the synapse and enhance dopaminergic neurotransmission, thereby stimulating locomotor activity and producing reward in animals. The fact that amphetamine produces locomotor inhibition instead of stimulation in DAT-KD mice suggested the possibility that cocaine effects might also be altered in these mice.
Fig. 1 shows that DAT-KD mice had considerably higher baseline locomotor activity than WT mice before drug or saline injection and this difference was statistically significant (Fig. 1C). This result CONFIRMS our previous observation [18] and is consistent with the hyperdopaminergic tone in these mice [18]. Fig. 1 also shows that cocaine clearly increased locomotor activity in DAT-KD mice as well as in WT mice. While cocaine was capable of increasing locomotor activity in both genotypes, there were differences in each genotype's response. For instance, both 5 and 10 mg/kg cocaine produced stronger locomotor stimulation in DAT-KD mice than it did in wild type mice (Fig. 1D). The reason for this difference is not clear but could be due to presynaptic or postsynaptic adaptive changes, such as reduced D2 autoreceptor signaling, in response to the hyperdopaminergic tone in DAT-KD mice. It is also not clear why the amphetamine effect is altered in DAT-KD mice while the cocaine effect is maintained. Our results demonstrate that a dramatically reduced DAT activity does not lead to a loss or a reduction of the stimulating effect of cocaine. This is in contrast to observations of DAT-KO mice and DAT-CI mice. In heterozygous DAT-KO mice, DAT activity is reduced by 50% and cocaine stimulates locomotion as well as in WT mice; while in homozygous DAT-KO mice, DAT is absent and cocaine has no effect on locomotion [24]. In DAT-CI mice, the mutated DAT is over 50-fold less sensitive to cocaine inhibition but has lower uptake activity. Instead of stimulation, cocaine suppresses locomotor activity in DAT-CI mice [17]. The fact that cocaine retains these effects in both heterozygous DAT-KO mice and DAT-KD mice indicates that the reduction of DAT expression and thus DAT activity to 50% and 10% of wild type mice does not by itself lead to adaptive changes that abolish the stimulating effect of cocaine and in fact, 10% DAT activity seems to be sufficient for the preservation of many normal functions, such as some cocaine responses. Therefore, the locomotor suppression observed in DAT-CI mice is not due to reduced DA clearance or changes in DA homeostasis but is due to the lack of cocaine inhibition of DAT and to cocaine effects on other cocaine targets. Cocaine inhibition of DAT, SERT, and NET and the resulting enhancement of neurotransmission in all three systems may contribute to the locomotor effect in wild type mice. Cocaine inhibition of SERT and/or NET in the absence of DAT inhibition in DAT-CI mice leads to locomotor suppression. It is not clear whether cocaine inhibition of SERT or NET enhances or dampens locomotor stimulation in wild type mice where DAT is also inhibited. Further studies are being performed to assess the role of SERT and NET in locomotor suppression.
Cocaine and other addictive drugs are known to produce reward in humans as well as in mice and other animals. CPP tests are commonly used to measure the rewarding properties of drugs. In this behavioral test, drug administration to animals is repeatedly paired with a set of environmental cues. When allowed to explore freely, the animals spend more time in the environment paired with a drug that produces reward.
Previously we have shown that up to 20 mg/kg cocaine is not enough to inhibit the cocaine-insensitive mutant DAT and elevate extracellular DA in DAT-CI mice [17]. We have also shown that cocaine lost its ability to produce reward in DAT-CI mice while amphetamine still produces CPP in these mice. Therefore, we conclude that the reward pathway in DAT-CI mice is functional and the lack of cocaine-induced reward in DAT-CI mice is due to the inability of cocaine to block the modified DAT. However, DAT activity in DAT-CI mice is significantly lower than that in WT mice. The DA clearance rate in DAT-CI mice is between those of DAT-KD mice and heterozygous DAT-KO mice [17]. It remains a concern that lowered DAT activity might lead to neuroadaptations that may alter cocaine effects. Thus, we examined whether cocaine is still able to produce reward in DAT-KD mice that have only 10% of wild type DAT expression. Fig. 2 shows that cocaine still produced robust CPP in DAT-KD mice. In addition, cocaine produces reward in heterozygous DAT-KO mice [9-11]. These results demonstrate that the reduction of DAT activity and elevated dopaminergic tone does not abolish cocaine's rewarding effect in mice.
Several mouse MODELS have been generated with alterations to the DAT gene, providing excellent tools to study the functions of DAT and the mechanism of cocaine effects: DAT-KD mice [18] and heterozygous DAT-KO mice [24] with DAT expression reduced to 10% and 50% of the wild type level respectively, DAT-CI mice [17] with a cocaine-insensitive DAT mutant that is functional but with reduced uptake activity, and homozygous DAT-KO mice [24] with DAT completely deleted. Heterozygous DAT-KO mice [24]and DAT-KD mice have significantly elevated DA tone but in these mice DAT is blocked by cocaine and the mice respond to cocaine similarly to wild type mice. The adaptive changes to the elevated dopaminergic tone in these mice are likely to be moderate. The changes do not abolish cocaine's ability to produce reward or to stimulate locomotor activity. Therefore, it is likely that the mechanism of cocaine action in these mice is similar to that in wild type mice.
In DAT-CI mice, DA uptake activity is reduced and the basal DA tone is also significantly elevated[17]. Since the extent of basal DA elevation in DAT-CI mice is between those in heterozygous DAT-KO and DAT-KD mice [17], it is reasonable to assume that the adaptive changes in DAT-CI mice are also moderate and do not interrupt the drug reward pathway. We have shown that amphetamine still produces reward in DAT-CI mice suggesting a functional reward pathway [17]. Although the elevated basal DA tone is due to reduced DAT activity in all mouse lines, the causes of the reduction are different; reduced expression of wild type DAT, in heterozygous DAT-KO and DAT-KD mice, versus normal expression of a mutant DAT with lower uptake activity in DAT-CI mice. Therefore, it is still possible that the mutated DAT in DAT-CI mice may cause unique adaptive changes, by an unknown mechanism, which could abolish cocaine reward and locomotor stimulation.
In contrast, in homozygous DAT-KO mice DAT is completely lacking. Intriguingly, the elimination of DAT, the consequent severe alteration of DA homeostasis and related pathways, and the tremendous adaptive changes still fail to abolish the rewarding effect of cocaine [24]. However, the mechanism of cocaine reward in DAT-KO mice is very different from that in wild type mice. For example, inhibition of SERT or NET with selective inhibitors elevates DA levels in the nucleus accumbens and produces reward in DAT-KO mice but not in wild type mice [24]. These results indicate that the complete deletion of DAT significantly alters the mechanism of cocaine effects. It seems that cocaine-induced DA elevation in the nucleus accumbens is still critical and the signaling from serotoninergic or noradrenergic systems to the dopaminergic system might be altered. The exact mechanism is not clear and currently under intensive investigation.
-------------------------------------------------
Conclusion
In summary, genetically modified mouse strains with a reduction in DAT function by 50% (heterozygous DAT-KO mice) and 90% (DAT-KD mice) retain the stimulating and rewarding effects of cocaine. These results are consistent with the idea that reduced DAT activity in DAT-CI mice does not lead to adaptive changes that abolish the stimulating and rewarding effects of cocaine. Therefore, the lack of cocaine reward and locomotor stimulation in DAT-CI mice is unlikely due to reduced DAT activity or the resulting hyperdopaminergic tone, but due to the inability of cocaine to inhibit the modified DAT. This investigation lends further support to the conclusion that cocaine inhibition of DAT is necessary for the rewarding and stimulating effects of cocaine.
-------------------------------------------------
Methods
Animals
The DAT-KD mice were generated by the insertion of a DNA construct containing the neomycin-resistant gene and other elements into the promoter region of DAT gene in an attempt to control DAT expression [18]. This promoter modification results in a 90% reduction in DAT expression [18]. Mice used in this study were produced from the breeders that have been backcrossed with C57BL/6J mice for 8–9 generations. Heterozygous male and FEMALE DAT-KD mice were bred to produce homozygous DAT-KD mice and their WT littermates used in the experiments. The genotypes of the mice used in these experiments were determined by PCR using forward primer TGGGGTCCACATACAAATGATGA and reverse primer ACACGTGGCAGATTCATAGGTA. Only male mice between 10 and 14 weeks of age were used in this study. Mice were group housed, with 3 – 5 mice per cage, on a 12 hour day/night cycle with lights off at 6 pm. Food and water was provided ad libitum. All procedures were conducted in accordance with the animal use committee at The Ohio State University. Experiments were performed between 9:00 am and 5:00 pm. Only drug naïve animals were used in experiments and separate animals were used for the locomotor and CPP studies.
Drugs
The drug supply program of National Institute on Drug Abuse, NIH (Bethesda, MD), kindly supplied cocaine HCL. Doses of 5 mg/kg, 10 mg/kg and 20 mg/kg were given for the locomotion studies and doses of 5 mg/kg and 20 mg/kg were used for the CPP studies. Vehicle (0.9% NaCl) and cocaine were administered through intraperitoneal (i.p.) injection.
Locomotion tests
Locomotion tests were performed in acrylic test chambers measuring 25 cm(W) × 25 cm(L) × 25 cm(H) that are inside a sound-attenuating behavioral test enclosure. Mice were placed in the chamber without drugs for 60 min. The total distance traveled during this time was used to determine the basal activity. Then cocaine or vehicle was administered and mouse locomotor activities were monitored for another 60 minutes. Cumulative distance traveled and speed was recorded by a video-based monitoring system, Limelight (Actimetrics, Evanston, IL). The stimulatory effects of cocaine peaked between 10 and 20 minutes, thus we used distance traveled within the first 30 minutes after injection as a measurement of cocaine effect on locomotion.
Conditioned place preference
CPP was performed as previously described [17]. In this setup, we used a three-chambered box with the two end chambers differing in wall patterns and FLOOR MATERIALS. One chamber had a thin wavy pattern on a white background on the walls and thin-mesh flooring, while the other chamber had a thick grid pattern on a white background on the walls and thick-mesh flooring. The middle chamber, used as a neutral initial placement point, had gray walls and a slick, acrylic floor. On the pre-conditioning day (day 1) mice were locked in the middle chamber for 30 seconds, then allowed to freely explore the entire apparatus for 30 min. Movement and time spent in each chamber was recorded with the video-based monitoring system LimeLight. Mice did not show a significant preference for either end chamber. Mice were randomly paired to one end chamber and randomly divided into saline control and cocaine-injected groups. On days 2, 4, 6, and 8, the mice were injected with saline and confined in one of the two end chambers for 30 min; and on days 3, 5, 7, and 9, the mice were injected with cocaine or saline and confined to the opposite end chamber for 30 minutes. On the test day (day 10), the mice were confined to the central chamber for 30 seconds, and then allowed free access to both end chambers for 30 min. The time spent in each of the end chambers was recorded. The difference between the time spent in the paired chamber on pre-conditioning day and time spent in the same chamber on post-conditioning day was calculated and presented.
Data analysis
The experimental data were analyzed by two-way ANOVA and post hoc Bonferroni tests using GraphPad PRISM (GraphPad Software, Inc. San Diego, CA)

Similar Documents

Free Essay

A Case Study on an Interpersonal Issue in a Family Situation from a Psychological Perspective

...A Case Study on an interpersonal issue in a family situation from a psychological perspective. By Andrew S. Heldt An Assignment for the Master of Theology on Personal and Social Psychology Year 2: Semester 2 Outline 1. Assignment Question…………………………………….…………………..1 2. Introduction………………………………………………….……………….1 3. Case study – Description…………..……………..………………………….1 a) Early Childhood…………...………………………………...………..1 b) Adolescence……………….………………………………..………….2 c) Church and family relationships…………………………………….3 4. Case study – Analysis ……………………………………………………….5 5. Ongoing Development…………….………………………………...…….....8 6. Further action………………………………………………………………..8 7. Conclusion……………………………………………………………………11 8. Bibliography……………………………………………………………..…..12 Assignment Question Describe a case in your ministerial context related to the following: b) Interpersonal issues in a family situation Try to identify the various components that are important to understand this situation from a psychological perspective, making sure that you refer to the relevant psychological theory. Analyse this case in the light of what you have studied in this course. Finally, explain what action you plan to take in this situation, giving your reasons. How might what you have studied help you intervene effectively in this situation. Introduction The area of psychology being vary vast, the scope of this paper is limited to the case study of an individual and the strained relationship within a family situation as occasioned...

Words: 5593 - Pages: 23

Free Essay

Buyology

...1 of 83 file:///D:/000004/Buy__ology.html 08/08/2009 10:45 2 of 83 file:///D:/000004/Buy__ology.html CONTENTS TITLE PAGE FOREWORD BY PACO UNDERHILL INTRODUCTION 1: A RUSH OF BLOOD TO THE HEAD The Largest Neuromarketing Study Ever Conducted 2: THIS MUST BE THE PLACE Product Placement, American Idol , and Ford’s Multimillion-Dollar Mistake 3: I’LL HAVE WHAT SHE’S HAVING Mirror Neurons at Work 4: I CAN’T SEE CLEARLY NOW Subliminal Messaging, Alive and Well 5: DO YOU BELIEVE IN MAGIC? Ritual, Superstition, and Why We Buy 6: I SAY A LITTLE PRAYER Faith, Religion, and Brands 7: WHY DID I CHOOSE YOU? The Power of Somatic Markers 8: A SENSE OF WONDER Selling to Our Senses 9: AND THE ANSWER IS… Neuromarketing and Predicting the Future 10: LET’S SPEND THE NIGHT TOGETHER Sex in Advertising 11: CONCLUSION Brand New Day APPENDIX ACKNOWLEDGMENTS NOTES BIBLIOGRAPHY ABOUT THE AUTHOR COPYRIGHT FOREWORD PACO UNDERHILL It was a brisk September night. I was unprepared for the weather that day, wearing only a tan cashmere sweater underneath my sports jacket. I was still cold from the walk from my hotel to the pier as I boarded the crowded cruise ship on which I was going to meet Martin Lindstrom for the first time. He had spoken that day at a food service conference held by the Gottlieb Duttweiler Institute, the venerable Swiss think tank, and David Bosshart, the conference organizer, was eager for us to meet. I had never heard of Martin ...

Words: 66056 - Pages: 265

Premium Essay

Buyology

...1 of 83 file:///D:/000004/Buy__ology.html 08/08/2009 10:45 2 of 83 file:///D:/000004/Buy__ology.html CONTENTS TITLE PAGE FOREWORD BY PACO UNDERHILL INTRODUCTION 1: A RUSH OF BLOOD TO THE HEAD The Largest Neuromarketing Study Ever Conducted 2: THIS MUST BE THE PLACE Product Placement, American Idol , and Ford’s Multimillion-Dollar Mistake 3: I’LL HAVE WHAT SHE’S HAVING Mirror Neurons at Work 4: I CAN’T SEE CLEARLY NOW Subliminal Messaging, Alive and Well 5: DO YOU BELIEVE IN MAGIC? Ritual, Superstition, and Why We Buy 6: I SAY A LITTLE PRAYER Faith, Religion, and Brands 7: WHY DID I CHOOSE YOU? The Power of Somatic Markers 8: A SENSE OF WONDER Selling to Our Senses 9: AND THE ANSWER IS… Neuromarketing and Predicting the Future 10: LET’S SPEND THE NIGHT TOGETHER Sex in Advertising 11: CONCLUSION Brand New Day APPENDIX ACKNOWLEDGMENTS NOTES BIBLIOGRAPHY ABOUT THE AUTHOR COPYRIGHT FOREWORD PACO UNDERHILL It was a brisk September night. I was unprepared for the weather that day, wearing only a tan cashmere sweater underneath my sports jacket. I was still cold from the walk from my hotel to the pier as I boarded the crowded cruise ship on which I was going to meet Martin Lindstrom for the first time. He had spoken that day at a food service conference held by the Gottlieb Duttweiler Institute, the venerable Swiss think tank, and David Bosshart, the conference organizer, was eager for us to meet. I had never heard of Martin ...

Words: 66056 - Pages: 265

Premium Essay

The Secret of Language Leadership

...SECRET LANGUAGE of • HOW LEADERS INSPIRE ACTION THROUGH NARRATIVE The LEADERSHIP STEPHEN DENNING John Wiley & Sons, Inc. More Praise for The Secret Language of Leadership “Out of the morass of strategies leaders are given to transform organizations, Denning plucks a powerful one—storytelling— and shows how and why it works.” —Dorothy Leonard, William J. Abernathy Professor of Business, Emerita, Harvard Business School, and author, Deep Smarts: How to Cultivate and Transfer Enduring Business Wisdom “The Secret Language of Leadership shows why narrative intelligence is central to transformational leadership and how to harness its power.” —Carol Pearson, director, James MacGregor Burns Academy of Leadership, University of Maryland, and coauthor, The Hero and the Outlaw “The Secret Language of Leadership is not only the best analysis I have seen of how and why leaders succeed or fail, it’s highly readable, as well as downright practical. It should be mandatory reading for anyone interested in engaging a company with big ideas who understands that leaders live and die by the quality of what they say.” —Richard Stone, story analytics master, i.d.e.a.s “A primary role of leaders is to create and maintain meaning for their organizations. Denning clearly demonstrates that meaningmaking comes from stories well told.” —Thomas Davenport, President’s Distinguished Professor of I.T. and Management, Babson College, and author, The Attention Economy “Steve...

Words: 100587 - Pages: 403

Premium Essay

Myths

...Contents Preface Acknowledgments Introduction 1 BRAIN POWER Myth #1 Most People Use Only 10% of Their Brain Power Myth #2 Some People Are Left-Brained, Others Are Right-Brained Myth #3 Extrasensory Perception (ESP) Is a Well-Established Scientific Phenomenon Myth #4 Visual Perceptions Are Accompanied by Tiny Emissions from the Eyes Myth #5 Subliminal Messages Can Persuade People to Purchase Products 2 FROM WOMB TO TOMB Myth #6 Playing Mozart’s Music to Infants Boosts Their Intelligence Myth #7 Adolescence Is Inevitably a Time of Psychological Turmoil Myth #8 Most People Experience a Midlife Crisis in | 8 Their 40s or Early 50s Myth #9 Old Age Is Typically Associated with Increased Dissatisfaction and Senility Myth #10 When Dying, People Pass through a Universal Series of Psychological Stages 3 A REMEMBRANCE OF THINGS PAST Myth #11 Human Memory Works like a Tape Recorder or Video Camera, and Accurate Events We’ve Experienced Myth #12 Hypnosis Is Useful for Retrieving Memories of Forgotten Events Myth #13 Individuals Commonly Repress the Memories of Traumatic Experiences Myth #14 Most People with Amnesia Forget All Details of Their Earlier Lives 4 TEACHING OLD DOGS NEW TRICKS Myth #15 Intelligence (IQ) Tests Are Biased against Certain Groups of People My th #16 If You’re Unsure of Your Answer When Taking a Test, It’s Best to Stick with Your Initial Hunch Myth #17 The Defining Feature of Dyslexia Is Reversing Letters Myth #18 Students Learn Best When Teaching Styles Are Matched to...

Words: 130018 - Pages: 521

Free Essay

Test2

...62118 0/nm 1/n1 2/nm 3/nm 4/nm 5/nm 6/nm 7/nm 8/nm 9/nm 1990s 0th/pt 1st/p 1th/tc 2nd/p 2th/tc 3rd/p 3th/tc 4th/pt 5th/pt 6th/pt 7th/pt 8th/pt 9th/pt 0s/pt a A AA AAA Aachen/M aardvark/SM Aaren/M Aarhus/M Aarika/M Aaron/M AB aback abacus/SM abaft Abagael/M Abagail/M abalone/SM abandoner/M abandon/LGDRS abandonment/SM abase/LGDSR abasement/S abaser/M abashed/UY abashment/MS abash/SDLG abate/DSRLG abated/U abatement/MS abater/M abattoir/SM Abba/M Abbe/M abbé/S abbess/SM Abbey/M abbey/MS Abbie/M Abbi/M Abbot/M abbot/MS Abbott/M abbr abbrev abbreviated/UA abbreviates/A abbreviate/XDSNG abbreviating/A abbreviation/M Abbye/M Abby/M ABC/M Abdel/M abdicate/NGDSX abdication/M abdomen/SM abdominal/YS abduct/DGS abduction/SM abductor/SM Abdul/M ab/DY abeam Abelard/M Abel/M Abelson/M Abe/M Aberdeen/M Abernathy/M aberrant/YS aberrational aberration/SM abet/S abetted abetting abettor/SM Abeu/M abeyance/MS abeyant Abey/M abhorred abhorrence/MS abhorrent/Y abhorrer/M abhorring abhor/S abidance/MS abide/JGSR abider/M abiding/Y Abidjan/M Abie/M Abigael/M Abigail/M Abigale/M Abilene/M ability/IMES abjection/MS abjectness/SM abject/SGPDY abjuration/SM abjuratory abjurer/M abjure/ZGSRD ablate/VGNSDX ablation/M ablative/SY ablaze abler/E ables/E ablest able/U abloom ablution/MS Ab/M ABM/S abnegate/NGSDX abnegation/M Abner/M abnormality/SM abnormal/SY aboard ...

Words: 113589 - Pages: 455

Free Essay

La Singularidad

...NOTE: This PDF document has a handy set of “bookmarks” for it, which are accessible by pressing the Bookmarks tab on the left side of this window. ***************************************************** We are the last. The last generation to be unaugmented. The last generation to be intellectually alone. The last generation to be limited by our bodies. We are the first. The first generation to be augmented. The first generation to be intellectually together. The first generation to be limited only by our imaginations. We stand both before and after, balancing on the razor edge of the Event Horizon of the Singularity. That this sublime juxtapositional tautology has gone unnoticed until now is itself remarkable. We're so exquisitely privileged to be living in this time, to be born right on the precipice of the greatest paradigm shift in human history, the only thing that approaches the importance of that reality is finding like minds that realize the same, and being able to make some connection with them. If these books have influenced you the same way that they have us, we invite your contact at the email addresses listed below. Enjoy, Michael Beight, piman_314@yahoo.com Steven Reddell, cronyx@gmail.com Here are some new links that we’ve found interesting: KurzweilAI.net News articles, essays, and discussion on the latest topics in technology and accelerating intelligence. SingInst.org The Singularity Institute for Artificial Intelligence: think tank devoted to increasing...

Words: 237133 - Pages: 949