Free Essay

The Gaze Experiment: a Study of Gazing Habits and Biases with Respect to Different Facial Features.

In:

Submitted By chewyyo
Words 2231
Pages 9
The Gaze Experiment: A study of gazing habits and biases with respect to different facial features.
AbstractCan we tell which part of our face a person is gazing at when they are gazing at us? The experiment in question gives us an idea to where and why we think a person is looking at that location. There were 27 students split into pairs. Within the pair there was an experimenter and a participant. The experimenter would look at different parts of the participant's face and ask the participant to tell them where they were looking at. Our results showed us that the participants were most accurate at recognising when the eyes, hair, right and left ears were being looked at with correct responses ranging between 46.3%-51.85%. And least accurate when the nose, mouth and chin were being looked at with correct responses ranging between 22.59%-25.19%. Percentage incorrect responses was highest for eyes at 30.1%. The eyes, hair and ears were the easiest to be recognised because those features are the furthest away from other facial parts. The eyes have the highest incorrect response percentage because most people assume it is their eyes people are looking at during social interaction so if the participant was unsure where the experimenter was looking at, the guess would have likely been the eyes.

IntroductionNon-verbal communication between humans was first scientifically investigated in 1872 where Charles Darwin wrote “The Expression of Emotions in Man and Animals”. Since then many scientists have investigated the topic. There are many different non-verbal channels of communication. Michael Argyle (1988, 1994) suggested that facial expressions, gaze, voice, gesture, posture amongst others were channels of non-verbal communication.
One of the most widely studied channels are gazes. A similar experiment to ours was performed in 1963 by Gibson & Pick. They tested whether a participant could distinguish whether the experimenter was gazing at their eyes. They tested this by looking at the eyes of the participant and just outside their face and asking them to determine whether they were being stared in the eyes. They had another independent variable that we do not use in our experiment, the experimenter would stare at seven points on a horizontal line 10cm apart with the centre point being on the nose of the participant. They found that the participant could correctly distinguish whether their eye were being stared at 84% of the time. However when the experimenter wasn’t facing the participant head on, they found their success rate drop. The experiment showed that both head direction and pupil direction are important in determining where a person is gazing.
After Gibson & Pick, Von Cranach & Ellgring (1973) also performed a similar experiment to ours. They increased the number of variables that would affect a person's judgement about where they were being stared at, eg. head direction, gaze duration, number of points on the face. The testing of different points on the face is similar to our experiment. They found that the participants made the most errors perceiving that their eyes were being looked at when they were not. This is understandable as the eyes are where we would assume people who look at us are looking at. It daily human interaction, when peoples' gazes are fixed upon our faces we assume they are looking at our eyes.
As a result of Von Cranach & Ellgring's experiment we can predict that we would find that the eyes will have the highest number of incorrect responses, representing the fact that people assume they are being looked at in the eye because it is a social norm. From the combination of the two above experiments we can also predict that facial extremities like the chin, ears and hair will have higher correct responses as opposed to the nose and mouth. Gibson & Pick showed that when looking slightly outside the face, past the ears or hair or chin, we can tell that the experimenter is not looking at our eyes. And with Von Cranach & Ellgring's experiment we can assume that points within the face (nose and mouth) will be harder to for the participant to recognise.
MethodParticipants
There were 27 participants, five male and 22 female aged between 18-20 years old. The participants were drawn from students studying Introduction to Psychological Research at Durham University. Seven of the participants wear glasses, four wear contacts lenses and the remainder have uncorrected vision.
Apparatus
Each experimenter was given a gaze record sheet. The experimenter would had to note the gender and any visual aids the participant may have had. The gaze record sheet had a grid with T (target) and R (response) heading the columns. The rows would have different targets the experimenter had to look at and were filled by letters, each letter donated different target: E= eyes, N= nose, M= mouth, C=chin, H=hair, RE=right ear, LE=left ear. The response column would then be filled with the letter of which facial feature the participant was looking at.
Design
The experiment was a within-subjects design. The independent variable was the seven different facial targets the experimenter would look at. The two dependent variables are the percentage of correct responses and the percentage of incorrect responses or response bias of each facial target. The experimenter would random where they chose to start on the column of targets to get rid of fatigue effect and practice effect.
Procedure
The subjects will pair up, one taking the role of experimenter and the other participant. Any visual aids should be worn to give the subjects their best vision. The pair should sit facing each other 1.5m away from each other. The experimenter should place the record sheet in front of them and cover it so the participant cannot see it and fill out the participant's gender and visual aids (if any). The experimenter will then choose a random point on the column to start.
Once the experimenter has chosen, he/she should look to the ceiling and then lower their gaze to rest upon the facial target that the experimenter has randomly chose to start with. The participant should know to responds within five seconds, and if at the end of the five seconds the participant is still unsure he/she should guess. The participant will then say where they think the experimenter is looking at. The experimenter will then note down where the participant said in the response column next to the target the experimenter was looking at on the gaze record sheet using the letters that donate the facial features mentioned earlier. Continue with each target on the sheet until all have been completed. No feedback should be given during the trial. The experiment should be repeated with the participant and experimenter roles reversed.
ResultsWe totalled up the number of correct and incorrect responses for each facial part. In table 1 we take the total number of correct responses for each facial part and divide it by 270 (the number of people (27) multiplied by the number of times the target was presented (10)) and multiply it all by 100 to obtain a percentage. The percentage figures show us what percentage of the time do the participants provide a correct answer to when the facial part is gazed at. We can see that the hair, ears and eyes have the highest percentage of correct responses and the nose, mouth and chin have the lowest.
Table 1. Percentage of Correct Responses Percentage Correct Response | H | RE | E | LE | N | M | C | 51.85 | 51.11 | 46.30 | 48.89 | 24.07 | 25.19 | 22.59 |

Table 2 is slightly different, it represents the percentage of incorrect responses expressed in percentage of errors. We can see that the eyes, nose and mouth have relatively higher incorrect response rates. The ears, hair and chin have extremely low incorrect response rates relatively. The figures were calculated by dividing the total number of incorrect responses for each facial target by the total number of incorrect responses over all participants and targets, multiplied by 100.
Table 2. Percentage of Incorrect Responses Percentage Incorrect Response | H | RE | E | LE | N | M | C | 7.15 | 4.48 | 30.1 | 5.51 | 27.7 | 18 | 7.1 |

The diagram show us the mean response of correct answers for each facial part. We can see that most participants managed to recognise that their hair, ears and eyes were being looked at half the time. Whereas the bars for the nose, mouth and chin are about the half the height of the other facial parts showing us that participants were less sure of when those facial parts were being looked at, averaging slightly above two correct guesses each.

The next graph shows us a different pattern. The graph above clearly shows two distinct groups that the participants could identify at different competencies. The graph below shows us again two groups that the participants could identify at different competencies. The difference now is that the eyes, nose and mouth fall into one group and the ears, chin and hair fall into a separate group. The eyes, nose and mouth were identified incorrectly 2-3 times more than the ears, chin and hair facial parts.

DiscussionFrom our correct responses results we can infer that we can distinguish between the top facial features better than the bottom half of our face. This could be down to the fact that we can recognize horizontal eye movement better than vertical, or simply because there are fewer facial targets on a horizontal axis than the vertical therefore giving us a higher chance to guessing a correct target when we recognise a horizontal eye movement. There is another explanation why the results show a higher correct response rate for the upper half of our face. The facial targets on the upper half of our face are also further apart than the bottom half of our face which might explain why there is a higher correct response rate.
The incorrect responses results tells a different story. The facial extremities (hair, chin and ears) incorrect responses are 2-3 times lower than that of the facial targets within the face (nose, eyes and mouth). This relation could be explained by the fact that we stare people in the eyes by nature during social conversations so maybe we use it as a reference point. With the eyes, nose and mouth being a close group, the small pupil movements of the observer may not be noticed. Also these three facial targets are within the face where we may recognise being stared at with no difference between the targets. The facial targets on the extremities of the face boarder on being off the face and maybe that is why we can tell better. We can tell when someone is gazing just off our face pretty well from Gibson & Pick's research and this supports ours.
The target with the highest incorrect responses was the eyes. This is understandable as we would guess that it was the eyes that was being looked at when someone is looking at our facial area because it is a social norm to be looking at the eyes. This finding supports Von Cranach & Ellgring's experiment that eyes will have the highest incorrect responses. The only difference from our predictions is that we would assume the chin would have a higher correct response rate being on the edge of our face. I think this could be that the chin, mouth and nose are closely spaced and the chin unlike the other facial targets on the edge of the face has two targets between it- nose and mouth.
Our experiment was not entirely accurate. We didn’t measure exactly 1.5m between the experimenter and participant. Also if one of the pair was significantly shorter/taller than the other, the distance between the faces will have increased. On top of the increased distance we would have been adding an angled view of the other's face which would influence the result as shown in Gibson & Picks's results. There were a lot of targets that had to be viewed in a row, this meant that both/either or experimenter and participant would move their heads, shift positions in their seats, etc.
If we were to perform this experiment again, to control the extraneous variables of head movement, body movement, height and distance difference we can use a two head frames 1.5m apart where both the experimenter and participant will rest their heads. This will control the extraneous variables in our experiment. However by controlling these variables that occur naturally in real life social interactions we may actually be making the results less reliable. In real life people shift and move constantly when looking at each other and taking this away could make our experiment less ecologically valid.
ReferencesArgyle, M. (1994). The Psychology of Interpersonal Behaviour.

Gibson & Pick (1963). Perception of another person's looking behaviour. American Journal of Psychology, 76, 386-394.
Langton, S.R., Watt, R.J., & Bruce, V. (2000). Do the eyes have it? Cues to the direction of social attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4 (2), 50- 59.
Von Cranach, M., & Ellgring, J.H. (1973). In M. Von Cranach & I Vine (Eds.). Social communication and movement. London: Academic Press.

Similar Documents

Premium Essay

Public Speaking Book

...A BRIEF CONTENTS PART 1 • GETTING STARTED 1. Becoming a Public Speaker 2. From A to Z: Overview of a Speech 3. Managing Speech Anxiety 4. Ethical Public Speaking 5. Listeners and Speakers 1 2 8 1 4 23 30 PART 2 • DEVELOPMENT 6. Analyzing the Audience 7. Selecting a Topic and Purpose 8. Developing Supporting Material 9. Locating Supporting Material 10. Doing Effective Internet Research 1 Citing Sources in Your Speech 1. 36 37 49 57 64 73 83 PART 3 • ORGANIZATION 1 Organizing the Speech 2. 1 Selecting an Organizational Pattern 3. 1 Outlining the Speech 4. 92 93 103 1 10 PART 4 • STARTING, FINISHING, AND STYLING 15. Developing the Introduction and Conclusion 16. Using Language 1 22 1 23 1 31 PART 5 • DELIVERY 1 Choosing a Method of Delivery 7. 18. Controlling the Voice 19. Using the Body 1 39 1 40 1 44 1 48 PART 6 • PRESENTATION AIDS 20. Types of Presentation Aids 21. Designing Presentation Aids 22. A Brief Guide to Microsoft PowerPoint 154 155 161 164 PART 7 • TYPES OF SPEECHES 23. Informative Speaking 24. Persuasive Speaking 25. Speaking on Special Occasions 1 74 1 75 188 21 7 PART 8 • THE CLASSROOM AND BEYOND 230 26. Typical Classroom Presentation Formats 27. Science and Mathematics Courses 28. Technical Courses 29. Social Science Courses 30. Arts and Humanities Courses 31. Education Courses 32. Nursing and Allied Health Courses 33. Business Courses and Business Presentations 34. Presenting in Teams 35. Communicating in Groups 231 236 240 243 246 248 25 1 253 258...

Words: 104318 - Pages: 418

Premium Essay

Quiet: Power of Introverts

...MORE ADVANCE NOISE FOR QUIET “An intriguing and potentially lifealtering examination of the human psyche that is sure to benefit both introverts and extroverts alike.” —Kirkus Reviews (starred review) “Gentle is powerful … Solitude is socially productive … These important counterintuitive ideas are among the many reasons to take Quiet to a quiet corner and absorb its brilliant, thought-provoking message.” —ROSABETH MOSS KANTER, professor at Harvard Business School, author of Confidence and SuperCorp “An informative, well-researched book on the power of quietness and the 3/929 virtues of having a rich inner life. It dispels the myth that you have to be extroverted to be happy and successful.” —JUDITH ORLOFF, M.D., author of Emotional Freedom “In this engaging and beautifully written book, Susan Cain makes a powerful case for the wisdom of introspection. She also warns us ably about the downside to our culture’s noisiness, including all that it risks drowning out. Above the din, Susan’s own voice remains a compelling presence—thoughtful, generous, calm, and eloquent. Quiet deserves a very large readership.” —CHRISTOPHER LANE, author of Shyness: How Normal Behavior Became a Sickness 4/929 “Susan Cain’s quest to understand introversion, a beautifully wrought journey from the lab bench to the motivational speaker’s hall, offers convincing evidence for valuing substance over style, steak over sizzle, and qualities that are, in America, often derided. This book is brilliant...

Words: 118436 - Pages: 474

Premium Essay

Daraa

...USA $25.95 CANADA $27.95 • W h y do our headaches persist after taking a one-cent aspirin but disappear when we take a 50-cent aspirin? • Why does recalling the Ten Commandments reduce our tendency to lie, even when we couldn't possibly be caught? • W h y do we splurge on a lavish meal but cut coupons to save 25 cents on a can of soup? • W h y do we go back for second helpings at the unlimited buffet, even when our stomachs are already full? • And how did we ever start spending $4.15 on a cup of coffee when, just a few years ago, we used to pay less than a dollar? hen it comes to making decisions in our lives, we think we're in control. We think we're making smart, rational choices. But are we? In a series o f illuminating, often surprising experi­ ments, M I T behavioral economist Dan Ariely refutes the common assumption that we behave in fundamentally rational ways. Blending everyday experience with ground­ breaking research, Ariely explains how expectations, emotions, social norms, and other invisible, seemingly illogical forces skew our reasoning abilities. N o t only do we make astonishingly simple mistakes every day, but we make the same types of mistakes, Ariely discovers. We consistently overpay, underestimate, and procrastinate. We fail to understand the profound effects of our emotions on what we want, and we overvalue what we already own. Yet these misguided behaviors are neither random nor senseless. They're systematic and predict­ able—making us predictably irrational...

Words: 95122 - Pages: 381

Premium Essay

Guide-to-Managerial-Communication-9th.Pdf

...Guide to Managerial Communication Effective Business Writing and Speaking This page intentionally left blank Prentice Hall “Guide To” Series in Business Communication Guide to Managerial Communication Effective Business Writing and Speaking Ninth Edition Mary Munter Tuck School of Business Dartmouth College Prentice Hall Boston Columbus Indianapolis New York San Francisco Upper Saddle River Amsterdam Cape Town Dubai London Madrid Milan Munich Paris Montreal Toronto Delhi Mexico City Sao Paulo Sydney Hong Kong Seoul Singapore Taipei Tokyo Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Catalogue in Publication data available from the Library of Congress Editorial Director: Sally Yagan Editor in Chief: Eric Svendsen Acquisitions Editor: James Heine Director of Editorial Services: Ashley Santora Editorial Project Manager: Karin Williams Director of Marketing: Patrice Lumumba Jones Marketing Manager: Nikki Ayana Jones Marketing Assistant: Ian Gold Managing Editor: Central Publishing Project Manager: Debbie Ryan Production Project Manager: Clara Bartunek Creative Director: Jayne Conte Cover Designer: Karen Salzbach Cover Art: Getty Images, Inc. Media Editor: Denise Vaughn Media Project Manager: Lisa Rinaldi Full-Service Project Management: Aparna Yellai/PreMediaGlobal Composition: PreMediaGlobal Printer/Binder: Edwards Brothers Annex Cover Printer: Lehigh Phoenix Color Text Font: 10.5/12 Times New Roman Credits...

Words: 49486 - Pages: 198

Free Essay

.Development Stage

...e eBook Collection Scientific American-Psychology Appendix Ch01 This is a Protected PDF document. Please enter your user name and password to unlock the text. User Name: Password: Unlock Remember my user name and password. If you are experiencing problems unlocking this document or you have questions regarding Protectedpdf files please contact a Technical Support representative: In the United States: 1-877-832-4867 In Canada: 1-800-859-3682 Outside the U.S. and Canada: 1-602-387-2222 Email: technicalsupport@apollogrp.edu. molempien kuninkaasta pantiin rakenna suunnilleen valmiita kannatusta tunne vanhurskaus ranskan laupeutensa etko riitaa jutussa koyhaa huuto referenssit palvelijan seuduilla kuluu ainakin luulivat tuomari varmistaa vuoriston kansoihin poydan persian kommunismikerralla sinkut jokilaakson tekojaan uskovia kohtuullisen maara ostan kaytetty muidenkin hopeaa ylos eloon tavoin persian toimittaa saapuu totuuden kaytosta osoittamaan pelastuksen hanta palvelijasi toivonsa seitsemaksi juoda lahettanytrunsaasti osan julistan muutakin riemuitkaa mieleen samaan mielipiteesi galileastaoleellista valitus pienta pientapaattavat jo hehku hyvasteli hengella isieni joihin tasangon menevat kansaansa sano ratkaisuja kirjuri selaimilla tahdet kapitalismin hekin pahempia tuot sijasta kyllin hyvaan suomen liittyvista keskuuteenne etujen uskottavuus pysytteli tunnustanut tulkintoja lammasta sovitusmenot tahdot elaimet onkos keneltakaan otetaan silmansa laillista puhumme...

Words: 68177 - Pages: 273

Premium Essay

A Good E-Book on Various Religions Across the World

...THE HANDY RELIGION AN SWE R BOOK JOHN RENARD Detroit The Handy Religion Answer Book™ C O P Y R I G H T © 2002 BY VI S I B LE I N K PRE SS® This publication is a creative work fully protected by all applicable copyright laws, as well as by misappropriation, trade secret, unfair competition, and other applicable laws. No part of this book may be reproduced in any form without permission in writing from the publisher, except by a reviewer who wishes to quote brief passages in connection with a review written for inclusion in a magazine or newspaper. All rights to this publication will be vigorously defended. Visible Ink Press® 43311 Joy Rd. #414 Canton, MI 48187-2075 Visible Ink Press and The Handy Religion Answer Book are trademarks of Visible Ink Press LLC. Most Visible Ink Press books are available at special quantity discounts when purchased in bulk by corporations, organizations, or groups. Customized printings, special imprints, messages, and excerpts can be produced to meet your needs. For more information, contact Special Markets Director, Visible Ink Press, at www.visibleink.com or (734) 667-3211. Art Director: Mary Claire Krzewinski Typesetting: Graphix Group Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Renard, John, 1944The handy religion answer book / John Renard. p. cm. ISBN 1-57859-125-2 (pbk.) 1. Religions--Miscellanea. I. Title. BL80.2 .R46 2001 291--dc21 Printed in the United States of America All rights reserved ...

Words: 245202 - Pages: 981

Free Essay

Business

...CHAPTER 1 ASSESSING THE ENVIRONMENT POLITICAL, ECONOMIC, LEGAL, TECHNOLOGICAL LECTURE OUTLINE General Outline Opening Profile: India Becoming a Crucial Cog in the Machine at I.B.M. The Global Business Environment Management in Focus: A Small Company, A Global Approach Regional Trading Blocks Comparative Management in Focus: Opening Economy Revitalizes India Information Technology The Globalization of Human Capital The Global Manager’s Role The Political and Economic Environment The Legal Environment The Technological Environment Chapter Discussion Questions Application Exercises Experiential Exercise End-of-Chapter Case Study: Under Pressure, Dubai Company Drops Port Deal Additional Cases: India: The Employment Black Hole? Mecca Cola Student Stimulation Questions and Exercises Opening Profile: India Becoming a Crucial Cog in Machine at I.B.M. The opening profile reports on the growing importance of India as a source of low-cost services in the IT market. The Indian labor market is attractive not only due to its low wages, but also because of the scientific and managerial talent found in the country. IBM’s Indian facility in Bangalore is now the company’s second largest worldwide operation. While IBM has laid off thousands of workers in the United States, its Indian operation has greatly increased employment. Some of IBM’s competitors have also begun to move their operations to India. The opening profile raises the question of the...

Words: 96329 - Pages: 386

Free Essay

Psychoanalytic Personality Assessment

...e eBook Collection This is a Protected PDF document. Please enter your user name and password to unlock the text. User Name: Password: Unlock Remember my user name and password. If you are experiencing problems unlocking this document or you have questions regarding Protectedpdf files please contact a Technical Support representative: In the United States: 1-877-832-4867 In Canada: 1-800-859-3682 Outside the U.S. and Canada: 1-602-387-2222 Email: technicalsupport@apollogrp.edu. This document is published by:Apollo Group, Inc. ISBN 1-256-49231-0 joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka jokaGlossary joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka joka ...

Words: 153006 - Pages: 613